View Full Text | Abstract |
Article as PDF | Print this Article |
Pubmed | PMC |
PubReader | Export to Citation |
Email Alerts | Open Access |
Exp Neurobiol 2011; 20(3): 137-143
Published online September 30, 2011
https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2011.20.3.137
© The Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences
Hyun-Jung Park1#, Hyun Soo Shim2#, Woong Ki Choi1, Kyung Soo Kim1 and Insop Shim2#*
1Basic Oriental Medical Science and Acupuncture and Meridian Science Research Center, Kyung Hee University, Seoul 130-701, 2Department of Integrative Medicine and Research Center of Behavioral Medicine, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul 137-701, Korea
Correspondence to: #These authors equally contributed to this work.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed.
TEL: 82-2-961-0698, FAX: 82-2-957-0157
e-mail: ishim@khu.ac.kr
In order to the neuroprotective effect of
Keywords: trimethyltin (TMT), choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP), Lycium chinense fruit (LCF)
Trimethyltin (TMT) is a potent neurotoxicant which produces a dose-dependent degeneration of neurons in the limbic system (Dyer et al., 1982a, b; Chang and Dyer, 1983a; Earley et al., 1992), particularly the hippocampus, amygdale and entorhinal cortex (Dyer et al., 1982a, b; McMillan and Wenger, 1985; Balaban et al., 1988). The molecular basis of selective vulnerability of specific neuronal populations to neuronal insults has been a key focus in neurology and neuropathology (Brabeck et al., 2002). TMT-induced neurodegeneration is characterized by massive neuronal death, mainly localized in the limbic system, especially in the hippocampus, accompanied by reactive gliosis, epilepsy and marked neurobehavioral alterations, and is considered a useful model of neurodegeneration and selective neuronal death (Balaban et al., 1988; Koczyk et al., 1996b; Ishida et al., 1997; Brabeck et al., 2002; Geloso et al., 2002, 2004).
In the rat, TMT impairs hippocampally-dependent behaviors, including passive avoidance (Walsh et al., 1982a), water maze (Hagan et al., 1988; Earley et al., 1992) and working and/or reference memory measured in the radial arm maze (Walsh et al., 1982b; Miller and O'Callaghan, 1984; Bushnell and Angell, 1992; Alessandri et al., 1994). Furthermore, behavioral studies have shown increased locomotor activity, disruption in self-grooming and learning deficits in TMT-intoxicated rats (Swartzwelder et al., 1981, 1982; Cohen et al., 1987; Hagan et al., 1988; Segal, 1988; Cannon et al., 1991, 1994 a, b; Woodruff et al., 1991; Earley et al., 1992; Messing et al., 1992; Alessandri et al., 1994). TMT intoxication impairs the acquisition of water maze and Biel maze (water avoidance) task as well as Hebb-Williams maze and radial arm maze performance (Walsh et al., 1982a, b; O'Connell et al., 1994a, b, 1996; Ishida et al., 1997). Moreover, TMT has been shown to produce effects on operant behavior, since TMT-intoxicated rats had higher lever pressing rats under a fixed-ratio schedule of food presentation (Swartzwelder et al., 1981) and TMT impaired the performance of differential reinforcement at low response rates in an operant schedule (Woodruff et al., 1991). These anatomical and behavioral findings have made TMT-intoxicated rats an attractive model for degenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, the most common cause of dementia (Woodruff et al., 1994).
Herbs have recently become attractive as health-beneficial foods (physiologically functional foods) and as a source material for the development of drugs. Herbal medicines derived from plant extracts are being utilized increasingly to treat a wide variety of clinical diseases, with relatively little knowledge regarding their modes of action (Matthews et al., 1999).
The present study was undertaken to evaluate the neuroprotective effect of
Male Sprague-Dawley rats weighting 250~280 g each were purchased from Samtaco Animal Corp. (Kyungki-do, Korea). The animals were allowed to acclimatize themselves for at least 7 days prior to the experimentation. The animals were housed in individual cages under light-controlled conditions (12/12-hr light/dark cycle) and at 23℃ room temperature. Food and water were available ad libitum. All experiments were approved by the catholic university institutional animal care and use committee. They were allowed at least 1week to adapt to their environment before the experiments.
The rats were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) with TMT (6.0 mg/kg, body weight) dissolved in 0.9% saline and then returned to their home cages. The rats were randomly divided into the following groups (n=8 per group): the naïve rat (Normal), TMT injection+Saline administered rat (Control), and TMT injection+LCF administered rat (LCF). Drug (400 mg/kg) was dissolved in saline and orally administered for two weeks after TMT injection. From the 15th after the injection of drug, the water maze test was performed for one week.
The swimming pool of the Morris water maze was a circular water tank 200 cm in diameter and 35 cm deep. It was filled to a depth of 21 cm with water at 23±2℃. A platform 15 cm in diameter and 20 cm in height was placed inside the tank with its top surface being 1.5cm below the surface of the water. The pool was surrounded by many cues that were external to the maze (D'Hooge and De deyn, 2001). A CCD camera was equipped with a personal computer for the behavioral analysis. Each rat was received four daily trials. For 6 consecutive days, the rats were tested with three acquisition tests. They also received retention tests on the 7th day. For the acquisition test, the rat was allowed to search for the hidden platform for 180s and the latency to escape onto the platform was recorded. The animals were trained to find the platform that was in a fixed position during 6 days for the acquisition test, and then for the retention test (at the 7th day), they received a 1 min probe trial in which the platform was removed from the pool. The intertrial interval time was 1 min. Performance of the test animals in each water maze trial was assessed by a personal computer for the behavioral analysis (S-mart program, Spain).
At the end of the behavioral observation, the animals were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and then perfused transcardially with 100 ml of saline, followed by 500 ml of a 4% solution of formaldehyde prepared in phosphate buffer. The brains were then removed, postfixed in the same fixative for two to three hours at 4℃ and then placed overnight at 4℃ in PBS containing 20% sucrose. On the following day, the brain was cut into coronal sections that were sliced to 30 µm-thicknesses. The primary rabbit polyclonal antibodies against the following specific antigen were used: Cholinacetyl transferase (ChAT, concentration 1 : 2,000; Cambridge Research Biochemicals, Wilmington, DE), cAMP (concentration 1 : 200; Cambridge Research Biochemicals, Wilmington, DE). The primary antibody was prepared at a dilution of 2,000x in 0.3% PBST, 2% normal rabbit serum and 0.001% kehole limpit hemocyanin (Sigma, CA, USA). The sections were incubated in the primary antiserum for 72 h at 4℃. After three more rinses in PBST, the sections were placed in Vectastain Elite ABC reagent (Vector laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for 2 h at room temperature. Following a further rinsing in PBS, the tissue was developed using diaminobenzadine (Sigma, CA, USA) as the chromogen. Images were captured using an Axio Vision 3.0 imaging systems (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) and processed in Adobe Photoshop. For measuring cells of ChAT, the grid was placed on CA1 and CA3 in the hippocampus areas according to the method of Paxinos et al. (1985). The number of cells was counted at 100x magnification using a microscope rectangle grid measuring 200×200 µm.
Statistical comparisons were done for the behavioral and histochemical studies using the one-way ANOVA, repeated measure of ANOVA, respectively and Tukey
Fig. 1A shows mean group latencies to reach to hidden platform in the Morris water maze for all groups for 6 days. The escape latency differed among the groups when the results were averaged over all the session. The control group showed a worse performance than did the normal group (at the Day 1, 2, respectively). There were no significant main effects, but there was a slight trend for a significant interaction effect on the distance traveled to reach the platform.
To examine the spatial memory of rats, the time spent swimming to the platform was compared and analysis is illustrated in Fig. 1B. The times spent to the platform were significantly different among the groups [F2,24=7.6, p<0.05]; the normal group spent more time around the platform than the control group (p<0.05 for the normal group). However, treatment of LCF was significantly increased the time spent around the platform (p<0.01).
The results of determining the ChAT immunoreative cells per section from different hippocampal formations are shown in Fig. 2. The number of ChAT neurons in the CA1 area was 19.8±4.6 in the normal group, 16.5.7±0.3 in the control group and 18.5±1.9 in the LCF group [
The results of determining the cAMP immunoreative cells per section from different hippocampal formations are shown in Fig. 3. The number of cAMP neurons in the CA1 area was 18.5±1.3 in the normal group, 18.0±1.8 in the control group and 21.0±1.95 in the LCF group [
The present study demonstrated that TMT injections produced severe deficits in rat performance in a Morris water maze along with signs of neurodegeration, including decreased ChAT and cAMP activity in the hippocampus. Treatment with LCF attenuated TMT-induced learning and memory deficits in the maze and had a protective effect against TMT-induced decrease in ChAT and cAMP positive neurons.
Intozication with trimethyltin (TMT) leads to profound behavioural and cognitive deficits in both humans (Fortemps et al., 1978) and experimental animals (Dyer et al., 1982a, b; Ishida et al., 1997). In rats, TMT induced the degeneration of pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus and cortical areas (pyriform cortex, entorhinal cortex, subiculum) connected to the hippocampus, but there is also neuronal loss in the association areas (Brown et al., 1979; Chang and Dyer, 1983a, b; Chang et al., 1983a, b; Chang and Dyer, 1985; Balaban et al., 1988; Koczyk et al., 1996a, b).
Futhermore, behavioural studies have shown increased disruption in memory and learning deficits in TMT-intoxicated rats (Swartzwelder et al., 1982; Andersson et al., 1995). TMT intoxication impairs the acquisition of water maze performance (Hagan et al., 1988; Segal, 1988; Earley et al., 1992). The Morris water maze is well-established paradigm for evaluating deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory and the MWM spatial learning task has been used in the validation of rodent models for neurocognitive disorders and for the evaluation of possible neurocognitive treatments (D'Hooge and De Deyn, 2001; Luine et al., 2003). The impairment in spatial learning produced by TMT in the current studies is consistent with previous reports of spatial learning impairments (Walsh et al., 1982a, b; Hagan et al., 1988; Earley et al., 1992; Alessandri et al., 1994). However, this study proved that spatial memory continued to improve in LCF group during the training days compared to the control group. Also, the data of spatial probe trial demonstrated that LCF protects against the TMT-induced decrease of the spatial retention, especially long-term memory. It has been previously reported that LCF has profound curative effects on improving the memory and cognitive function of AD-like animal model (Sun et al., 2003). Also, Deng et al showed that the treatment of
The neuroprotective effects of these herbal drugs on the central acetylcholine system were also examined by histochemistry of hippocampal neurons. The degeneration of the cholinergic innervation from the basal forebrain to the hippocampal formation in the temporal lobe is thought to be one of the factors determining the progression of memory decay, both during normal aging and AD (Wu et al., 1999). The best available marker for cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain is ChAT activity. ChAT synthesizes the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, basal forebrain and the cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. A significant reduction in ChAT activity in the postmortem brains of demented patients has been reported. In addition, there is a 20-50% decrease in ChAT activity in the hippocampus of the TMT-induced rats. However, LCF modulates the function of these neurons and plays a role in their maintenance by preventing the TMT-induced decrease in ChAT activity (Rabbani et al., 1997; Chen et al., 2000). The present results show that LCF exerts beneficial effects on cholinergic neurotransmission in the brain by increasing the hippocampus ChAT activities.
In the brain, activation of cAMP signaling occurs after stimulation of adenylyl cylases by stimulatory G-proteins after binding of an extracellular ligand to a G-protein-coupled receptor (GPCR) and by Ca2+ through the Ca2+ -binding protein calmodulin (Wang and Strom, 2003). Genetic and pharmacological studies provide strong evidence that the cAMP signaling pathway is crucial for learning and memory across species (Kandal, 2004; Skoulakis and Grammenoudi, 2006). However, critical questions remain about the potential of this pathway as a target for memory enhancement because of contradictory results from pharmacological and genetic studies. Our results showed that the levels of cAMP in the hippocampus had no significant difference among the groups. It has been previously reported that short-term memory may be dependent on cAMP signaling (Ahi et al., 2004) and that it may be enhanced after pharmacological immediate posttraining increases of hippocampal cAMP (Vianna et al., 2000). In contrast, by using our conditional system, there was no close relation between enhancement of memory and the change of cAMP expression in the hippocampus on TMT-induced rats.
In summary, treatment with LCF attenuated TMT-induced learning and memory deficits in the Morris water maze and had a protective effect against TMT-induced decreased in cholinergic neurons. LCF is thus a good candidate for further investigations that may ultimately result in its clinical use. Further work examining the effects of LCF activation on additional behavioral tasks will help to elucidate whether increasing cAMP signaling may also facilitate other types of memory.