Articles

  • KSBNS 2024

Article

Original Article

Exp Neurobiol 2016; 25(4): 156-162

Published online August 31, 2016

https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2016.25.4.156

© The Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences

Effects of the Female Estrous Cycle on the Sexual Behaviors and Ultrasonic Vocalizations of Male C57BL/6 and Autistic BTBR T+ tf/J Mice

Hyopil Kim, Junehee Son, Hyoungseob Yoo, Hakyoo Kim, Jihae Oh, DaeHee Han, Yoon Hwang and Bong-Kiun Kaang*

Laboratory of Neurobiology, School of Biological Sciences, College of Natural Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea

Correspondence to: *To whom correspondence should be addressed.
TEL: 82-2-880-7525, FAX: 82-2-884-9577
e-mail: kaang@ snu.ac.kr

Received: July 14, 2016; Revised: July 28, 2016; Accepted: August 4, 2016

A primary characteristic of autism, which is a neurodevelopmental disorder, is impaired social interaction and communication. Furthermore, patients with autism frequently show abnormal social recognition. In mouse models of autism, social recognition is usually assessed by examining same-sex social behavior using various tests, such as the three-chamber test. However, no studies have examined the ability of male mice with autism to recognize the estrous cycle of female partners. In this study, we investigated the sexual behaviors, especially mounting and ultrasonic vocal communication (USV), of BTBR T+ tf/J (BTBR) mice, which are used as a well-known mouse model of autism, when they encountered estrus or diestrus female mice. As expected, C57BL/6 mice mounted more female mice in the estrus stage compared with the diestrus stage. We found that BTBR mice also mounted more female mice in the estrus stage than female mice in the diestrus stage. Although the USV emission of male mice was not different between estrus and diestrus female mice in both strains, the mounting result implies that BTBR mice distinguish sexual receptivity of females.

Keywords: Autism, Social recognition, Estrous cycle, Sexual interaction, Ultrasonic vocalization

Most female mammals experience the estrous cycles, which are physiological cycles regulated by fluctuations of gonadotropic hormones. The cycle is usually divided into four stages, the proestrus, estrus, metestrus, and diestrus, according to the levels of sexual hormones, such as estrogen and progesterone [1,2]. The duration of each cycle varies among species and ranges from a few days to several months. For mice, the average cycle duration is about 4~5 days [3].

The estrous cycle affects many behavioral and psychiatric states, including mood, anxiety, stress responses, sexual receptivity, learning, and memory [4,5,6,7,8,9]. In particular, sexual receptivity is highest at a stage in which copulation is crucial for successful reproduction [10]. Furthermore, the sniffing and mounting behaviors of male mice, which are primary actions that occur during copulation, are more frequent in response to urine from estrous females compared with urine from metestrus or diestrus mice [11]. The findings of these experiments suggest that male mice can recognize the sexual receptivity of female mice.

However, until now, little is known about how abnormal conditions affect the responses of male mice to females in different estrous states. In particular, patients with autism, which is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a primary characteristic of impaired social interaction, frequently exhibit deficits in the perception of social contexts and recognition of feelings or emotional states of others [12,13,14,15].

Sexual interaction is obviously a kind of social interaction, and patients with autism manifest abnormal sexual behaviors. Some patients show inappropriate sexual behaviors in public places, and there are reports which show that sexual offenders are more likely to have autistic symptoms [16,17,18]. Not only human patients, but also autistic mouse models have been characterized by abnormal sexual behaviors. For example, the copulatory behavior was decreased and intromission latency was increased in Balb/c mice [19,20]. An environmental autistic mouse model, which is prenatally exposed to valproate, showed increased mounting [21]. Based on these abnormal sexual behaviors in autism mouse models, we wondered if male autistic mice could distinguish appropriately the estrous cycle of female mice during sexual interaction.

Thus, we investigated it using BTBR mice, which are a well-known autistic mouse model [22,23], comparing their behaviors with those of the highly social C57BL/6 (B6) mice. Specifically, we analyzed the mounting behaviors, and ultrasonic vocalization (USV), which is thought to be implicated in social communication and sexual interactions [24,25,26], during interactions of male and estrus or diestrus females

Mice

Eight to fifteen-week-old BTBR and C57BL/6 mice were used in all experiments. Mice were kept on a 12 h light/dark cycle, and the behavioral experiment was performed during the dark phase of the cycle. Food and water were provided ad libitum. The mice were kept in a clean rack at 24~25℃ and 50~60% humidity. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Seoul National University approved the animal protocols and experiments (SNU-130807-3-3). Before each experiment, the mice were placed on a shelf for 30 min for acclimatization. Male mice used in all experiments were sexually inexperienced before the tests.

Determining the estrous stages of female mice

The estrous stages of female mice were determined 1 h before each behavior experiment. Vaginal smears were flushed with 10 µl of saline 3~5 times and 2 µl of the fluid was spread on a slide glass. Images were then obtained using a light microscope.

Reciprocal sexual interaction

Male mice were individually housed in a standard cage for at least 7 days. On the test day, a male mouse and its home cage were brought into the test room, and the mouse was allowed to habituate for 30 min under dim light. After habituation, an estrus or diestrus female mouse was introduced into the cage, and the behavior of the male mouse was video recorded for 10 min. The numbers of mounting and sniffing incidents were manually counted by an examiner who was blind to the estrous stage of the females.

USV

A male mouse that had been caged alone at least for 7 days was brought in its home cage into the test room under dim light. The home cage was placed inside a sound-attenuating Styrofoam box for USV recording. A microphone (CM16/CMPA, Avisoft Bioacoustics e.K., Glienicke, Germany) was fitted onto a transparent lid with a hole in the center. The microphone was connected to a USV recording device (UltraSoundGate 116H, Avisoft Bioacoustics e.K.). After a 30-min habituation period, an estrus or diestrus female mouse was introduced into the cage, and the USV calls during sexual interactions were recorded for 5 min. The sampling rate was set at 250 kHz and the bit depth was formatted to 16 bits for recording by RECORDER Hardlock software program (Avisoft Bioacoustics e.K). The recorded data were processed with SASLab Pro software program (Avisoft Bioacoustics e.K.). Signals with frequencies less than 35 kHz and noise with vertical patterns were removed. Each call in the recorded data was automatically determined (Hold time: 10 ms, Overlap: 75%, Hamming window). Dominant frequency, bandwidth, and duration were determined automatically with SASLab.

We examined if BTBR mice, which are used as a well-known animal model of autism, respond differently to the different estrous stage of females. Thus, we determined the estrous stage of adult female mice (B6 or BTBR) by examining vaginal smears [1,27]. During diestrus, which involves the regression of the corpus luteum, leukocytes were dominant, and little epithelial cells were observed (Fig. 1A). During proestrus, when the follicles in the ovaries begin to grow, nucleated epithelial cells were dominant, and leukocytes were still present (Fig. 1B). During estrus, when the female is sexually receptive, mostly cornified cells were observed (Fig. 1C). During metestrus, cornified epithelial cells and leukocytes were present (Fig. 1D). We chose to examine the estrus and diestrus stages in this study because the preferences for male urine and lordosis behavior are considerably decreased during diestrus than during estrus [28,29,30].

The total mounting duration for 10 min was analyzed, and as expected, the mounting time of male B6 mice was significantly higher with estrus females than with diestrus females (with estrus: n=13, with diestrus: n=9, student's t-test, *p<0.05, Fig. 2A).

None of the eight male BTBR mice that we tested mounted diestrus female partner at all whereas some mice (5 out of 9 mice) mounted estrus females (Table 1). In BTBR mice, the proportion of mounting was significantly higher with estrus females than with diestrus females (Fisher's exact test, *p<0.05, Fig. 2A). These results suggested that BTBR mice could recognize the sexual state of its mating partner. It seems that the mounting duration of BTBR was less than that of B6, although the tendency did not reach statistical significance (two-way ANOVA, strain x estrous cycle, strain: p=0.153, Fig. 2A). Sniffing behavior, which is a more general social behavior compared with mounting, was not affected by the female estrous cycle in either B6 or BTBR mice (Fig. 2B).

Next, we investigated if the patterns of USV emissions by male mice were affected by the female estrous cycle. We analyzed the total numbers and latencies for the first call of the USV that was emitted by male mice during 5 min of sexual interaction. However, both the numbers and first call latencies of the USV of male B6 mice did not differ between those matched with estrus females and diestrus females (Fig. 3A~C). The parameters of BTBR mice were also not affected by the female estrous cycle, but the total number and first call latencies of the USVs tended to be higher in BTBR mice compared with B6 mice (two-way ANOVA, strain x estrous cycle, strain: *p<0.05, Fig. 3B, C). We then analyzed specific properties, such as dominant frequency, bandwidth, and duration, of each USV call as the parameters of male USV calls. However, the parameters of each USV calls in both strains did not differ between estrus and diestrus females (Fig. 3D~F).

The receptivity of female mice is the highest in the estrus stage, and sexual behaviors, including mounting of male mice, are most frequent with estrus females [10,11]. We investigated the sexual interactions of male B6 and BTBR mice with female mice at different estrous stages. Particularly, the mounting durations with estrus female were increased compared to those with diestrus females in both strains. Although the mounting duration of BTBR tended to be lower compared with B6, both strains of mice distinguished the different sexual states of the females according to the estrous cycle. This suggested that BTBR mice could somewhat recognize different sexual states of female mice affected by estrous cycle. However, in order to examine if estrous cycle itself could be recognized by BTBR mice excluding the effects of the sexual receptive behavior of female mice, urine from females of different estrous stages should be used.

Sniffing was not affected by the estrous cycle in either strain. This behavior is a more general social behavior compared to mounting, and it is presented in many situations, including same-sex social interactions and interactions under nonsocial contexts.

USVs are composed of various call types although their meanings are not yet clear. USVs are associated with sexual behaviors. Mice with autism, including BTBR mice, exhibit abnormal patterns of USV emissions [23,31,32,33]. However, the USV call number and latency were not affected by the estrous cycle in either strain in our study. Thus, although USVs are involved with sexual behavior, they do not appear to be affected by the estrous cycle. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that a specific type of USV call may be influenced by the estrous cycle.

In contrast to the findings of a previous report [23], our results showed that the total number of USV calls in BTBR mice was not less than that of B6 mice. However, in the previous study, male mice were group-housed, and male mice encountered the females in cages with fresh bedding, which represented a novel environment. However, in the present study, male mice encountered the females in their own cages where they had been housed for at least 7 days. These methodological differences might explain the different results in the studies.

It would be interesting to determine how autistic BTBR mice can recognize the sexual states of females. One possible explanation is the contribution of oxytocin, a neuropeptide that has been implicated in pair bonding and sexual interactions [34]. Because oxytocin level was even higher in BTBR mice than B6 mice [35], elevated oxytocin might make BTBR mice distinguish the sexual states of females, in spite of their autistic phenotypes. Further studies should be done to understand the molecular mechanisms involved in the recognition of sexual states of female mice. Finally, as there are many autistic mouse models, our findings of sexual behaviors in BTBR cannot be generalized to all autistic model mice. Therefore, it would be interesting to compare the estrous cycle-dependent sexual behaviors among different autistic mouse lines.

Fig. 1. Classification of estrous cycles of female mice. (A) Diestrus. The leukocytes (yellow arrow) are dominant, and epithelial cells are rare. (B) Proestrus. Nucleated epithelial cells (red arrow) are dominant, and leukocytes are still present. (C) Estrus. Mostly cornified cells are observed. (D) Metestrus. Cornified epithelial cells (cyan arrow) with leukocytes are observed.
Fig. 2. Comparison of sexual behaviors exhibited by B6 and BTBR mice with females in the estrus and diestrus stages of the estrous cycle. (A) Total mounting duration of B6 and BTBR male mice. The mounting duration of B6 mice is higher with estrus females than with diestrus females (with estrus: n=13, with diestrus: n=9, Student's t-test, *p<0.05). Mounting behavior is not seen in BTBR male mice with diestrus females. The number of BTBR mice showing mounting is significantly higher with estrus females than with diestrus females (with estrus: 5/9, with diestrus: 0/8, Fisher's exact test, *p<0.05). (B) Total sniffing durations of both B6 and BTBR mice. The durations are not affected by the estrous cycle (B6, with estrus: n=13, with diestrus: n=9; BTBR, with estrus: n=9, with diestrus: n=8, Student t-test, p>0.05). All values are presented as mean±SEM.
Fig. 3. Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) of male B6 and BTBR mice with females in the estrus and diestrus stages. (A) Representative examples of USV calls. (B, C) Total number or latency to the first call of the USV of male B6 and BTBR mice with estrus or diestrus female. The number and latency of the calls with estrus or diestrus female do not differ (B6, with estrus: n=8, with diestrus: n=8; BTBR, with estrus: n=5, with diestrus: n=5, Student t-test, p>0.05). The number and latency tend to be higher in BTBR mice compared with B6 mice (two-way ANOVA, strain x estrous cycle, strain: *p<0.05). (D~F) The properties of each USV call (dominant frequency, bandwidth, and duration) do not differ (B6, with estrus: n=8, with diestrus: n=8; BTBR, with estrus: n=5, with diestrus: n=5, Student t-test, p>0.05). All values are presented as mean±SEM.
Table. 1. The number of male mice in B6 (A) and BTBR (B) that mounted or did not mount on each estrous cycle
B6BTBR
With estrusWith diestrusWith estrusWith diestrus
Mounting11850
No mounting2148
p value (Fisher's exact test)p>0.05p=0.0294

  1. Marcondes FK, Bianchi FJ, Tanno AP. Determination of the estrous cycle phases of rats: some helpful considerations. Braz J Biol 2002;62:609-614.
    Pubmed
  2. Pi X, Voogt JL. Sex difference and estrous cycle: expression of prolactin receptor mRNA in rat brain. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 2002;103:130-139.
    Pubmed
  3. Byers SL, Wiles MV, Dunn SL, Taft RA. Mouse estrous cycle identification tool and images. PLoS One 2012;7:e35538.
    Pubmed
  4. Conrad CD, Jackson JL, Wieczorek L, Baran SE, Harman JS, Wright RL, Korol DL. Acute stress impairs spatial memory in male but not female rats: influence of estrous cycle. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2004;78:569-579.
    Pubmed
  5. Schneider T, Popik P. Attenuation of estrous cycle-dependent marble burying in female rats by acute treatment with progesterone and antidepressants. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007;32:651-659.
    Pubmed
  6. Mora S, Dussaubat N, Díaz-Véliz G. Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on behavioral indices of anxiety in female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996;21:609-620.
    Pubmed
  7. Roberts DC, Bennett SA, Vickers GJ. The estrous cycle affects cocaine self-administration on a progressive ratio schedule in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;98:408-411.
    Pubmed
  8. González Jatuff AS, Torrecilla M, Quercetti M, Zapata MP, Rodríguez Echandía EL. Influence of the estrous cycle on some non reproductive behaviors and on brain mechanisms in the female rat. Interdisciplinaria 2012;29:63-77.
  9. Muhie S, Gautam A, Meyerhoff J, Chakraborty N, Hammamieh R, Jett M. Brain transcriptome profiles in mouse model simulating features of post-traumatic stress disorder. Mol Brain 2015;8:14.
    Pubmed
  10. Bronson FH. In: Bronson FH. Mammalian reproductive biology. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press, 1989; 1989. p. 67-75.
  11. Ingersoll DW, Weinhold LL. Modulation of male mouse sniff, attack, and mount behaviors by estrous cycle-dependent urinary cues. Behav Neural Biol 1987;48:24-42.
    Pubmed
  12. Lim CS, Yang JE, Lee YK, Lee K, Lee JA, Kaang BK. Understanding the molecular basis of autism in a dish using hiPSCs-derived neurons from ASD patients. Mol Brain 2015;8:57.
    Pubmed
  13. Boraston Z, Blakemore SJ, Chilvers R, Skuse D. Impaired sadness recognition is linked to social interaction deficit in autism. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:1501-1510.
    Pubmed
  14. Bradshaw J, Shic F, Chawarska K. Brief report: face-specific recognition deficits in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2011;41:1429-1435.
    Pubmed
  15. Williams BT, Gray KM. The relationship between emotion recognition ability and social skills in young children with autism. Autism 2013;17:762-768.
    Pubmed
  16. Coshway L, Broussard J, Acharya K, Fried K, Msall ME, Lantos JD, Nahata L. Medical therapy for inappropriate sexual behaviors in a teen with autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics 2016;137:e20154366.
    Pubmed
  17. Van Bourgondien ME, Reichle NC, Palmer A. Sexual behavior in adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1997;27:113-125.
    Pubmed
  18. Baarsma ME, Boonmann C, 't Hart-Kerkhoffs LA, de Graaf H, Doreleijers TA, Vermeiren RR, Jansen LM. Sexuality and autistic-like symptoms in juvenile sex offenders: a follow-up after 8 years. J Autism Dev Disord 2016;46:2679-2691.
    Pubmed
  19. Crawley JN, Belknap JK, Collins A, Crabbe JC, Frankel W, Henderson N, Hitzemann RJ, Maxson SC, Miner LL, Silva AJ, Wehner JM, Wynshaw-Boris A, Paylor R. Behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains: implications and recommendations for molecular studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;132:107-124.
    Pubmed
  20. Brodkin ES. BALB/c mice: low sociability and other phenotypes that may be relevant to autism. Behav Brain Res 2007;176:53-65.
    Pubmed
  21. Raza S, Himmler BT, Himmler SM, Harker A, Kolb B, Pellis SM, Gibb R. Effects of prenatal exposure to valproic acid on the development of juvenile-typical social play in rats. Behav Pharmacol 2015;26:707-719.
    Pubmed
  22. McFarlane HG, Kusek GK, Yang M, Phoenix JL, Bolivar VJ, Crawley JN. Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes Brain Behav 2008;7:152-163.
    Pubmed
  23. Scattoni ML, Ricceri L, Crawley JN. Unusual repertoire of vocalizations in adult BTBR T+tf/J mice during three types of social encounters. Genes Brain Behav 2011;10:44-56.
    Pubmed
  24. Portfors CV. Types and functions of ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats and mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2007;46:28-34.
    Pubmed
  25. Zhong J, Liang M, Akther S, Higashida C, Tsuji T, Higashida H. c-Fos expression in the paternal mouse brain induced by communicative interaction with maternal mates. Mol Brain 2014;7:66.
    Pubmed
  26. Barthelemy M, Gourbal BE, Gabrion C, Petit G. Influence of the female sexual cycle on BALB/c mouse calling behaviour during mating. Naturwissenschaften 2004;91:135-138.
    Pubmed
  27. Champlin AK, Dorr DL, Gates AH. Determining the stage of the estrous cycle in the mouse by the appearance of the vagina. Biol Reprod 1973;8:491-494.
    Pubmed
  28. Beach FA. Sexual attractivity, proceptivity, and receptivity in female mammals. Horm Behav 1976;7:105-138.
    Pubmed
  29. Yano S, Sakamoto KQ, Habara Y. Estrus cycle-related preference of BALB/c female mice for C57BL/6 males is induced by estrogen. J Vet Med Sci 2012;74:1311-1314.
    Pubmed
  30. Mossman CA, Drickamer LC. Odor preferences of female house mice (Mus domesticus) in seminatural enclosures. J Comp Psychol 1996;110:131-138.
    Pubmed
  31. Hanson JL, Hurley LM. Female presence and estrous state influence mouse ultrasonic courtship vocalizations. PLoS One 2012;7:e40782.
    Pubmed
  32. Takahashi N, Kashino M, Hironaka N. Structure of rat ultrasonic vocalizations and its relevance to behavior. PLoS One 2010;5:e14115.
    Pubmed
  33. Wöhr M. Ultrasonic vocalizations in Shank mouse models for autism spectrum disorders: detailed spectrographic analyses and developmental profiles. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014;43:199-212.
    Pubmed
  34. Lee SW, Kim YB, Kim JS, Kim WB, Kim YS, Han HC, Colwell CS, Cho YW, In Kim Y. GABAergic inhibition is weakened or converted into excitation in the oxytocin and vasopressin neurons of the lactating rat. Mol Brain 2015;8:34.
    Pubmed
  35. Silverman JL, Yang M, Turner SM, Katz AM, Bell DB, Koenig JI, Crawley JN. Low stress reactivity and neuroendocrine factors in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Neuroscience 2010;171:1197-1208.
    Pubmed