Articles

  • KSBNS 2024

Article

Short Communication

Exp Neurobiol 2017; 26(6): 390-398

Published online December 31, 2017

https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2017.26.6.390

© The Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences

Maternal Separation Does Not Produce a Significant Behavioral Change in Mice

Shawn Tan1,2†, Hin San Ho1,2†, Anna Yoonsu Song1,3, Joey Low1,4 and Hyunsoo Shawn Je1,2*

1Molecular Neurophysiology Laboratory, Signature Program in Neuroscience and Behavioral Disorders, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore, 169857, 2Department of Physiology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117597, 3College of Liberal Studies, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea, 4School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, 637551

Correspondence to: *To whom correspondence should be addressed.
TEL: 65-6601-1260, FAX: 65-6557-0729
e-mail: shawn.je@duke-nus.edu.sg
These authors contributed equally to this work.

Received: November 20, 2017; Revised: December 7, 2017; Accepted: December 8, 2017

Early life adversities together with genetic predispositions have been associated with elevated risks of neuropsychiatric disorders during later life. In order to investigate the underlying mechanisms, many chronic, early-life stress paradigms in multiple animal models have been developed. Previously, studies reported that maternal separation (MS) in the early postnatal stages triggers depression-and/or anxiety-like behaviors in rats. However, similar studies using mice have reported inconsistent behavioral outcomes. In this study, we sought to assess behavioral outcomes from two different early-life stress paradigms; a conventional 3-hour MS and a maternal separation with early weaning (MSEW) paradigm using C57BL/6J male mice with independent cohorts. Our data demonstrated that both MS and MSEW paradigms did not produce reported behavioral anomalies. Therefore, MS paradigms in mice require further validation and modification.


Keywords: Early-life stress, Maternal separation, Maternal separation with early weaning, Depression, Anxiety, Emotional memories

Early-life aversive experiences including childhood abuse, trauma, and neglect have been associated with increased risk for neuropsychiatric disorders, accompanied by potential neurophysiological changes such as the reduced volume and abnormal activation in the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex [1,2,3,4,5]. To identify underlying mechanisms of early-life, stress-induced brain dysfunction, maternal separation (MS) paradigms were developed in rodents [6,7]. In typical maternal separation paradigms, pups undergo 3 hours or more episodes of separation from the dam daily, which has been linked to increased levels of stress hormones and behavioral changes in adult animals [8,9]. While maternal separation paradigms in rat models are relatively well-established, behavioral effects upon MS paradigms in mice are often inconsistent (Table 1) [10,11], which may be due to subtle differences among separation paradigms, animal handling, or housing environments.

Here, we employed two different, early-life stress paradigms – a conventional protocol with 3-hour separation from P2 to P14 (MS) and a relatively recent protocol with longer time for maternal separation together with early weaning at P17 (MSEW) in C57BL/6J mice [12]. Following the two different paradigms, animals underwent multiple behavioral tests relevant to depression-like and anxiety-like behaviors and cognitive functions at P60. We hypothesized that the MSEW paradigm would result in increased severity in anxiety- and depressive-like behavior as well as enhanced contextual and cue fear learning as reported [12,13]. Intriguingly, we could not observe the predicted behavioral anomalies upon either MS or MSEW paradigm.

Animals

C57BL/6J breeding pairs were housed in a specific pathogen-free environment and maintained at 22℃ with 55% humidity. Food and water were provided ad libitum on a 12-hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 0700 h). Breeding pairs were checked daily for litters. If birth occurred, age was designated as P0. All the pups from one dam (a litter) were randomly assigned to one of the groups. Upon weaning, littermates were group-housed (up to 4 per cage). All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals for scientific purposes with approved protocols from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Duke-NUS Medical School (Protocol number: 2014/SHS/999). All tests were conducted on the same cohorts of animals. The MS cohort originally consisted of 9 MS and 8 MC mice while the MSEW cohort was comprised of 9 MSEW mice and 12 MC mice. During the course of testing, one cage in the MC group in the MSEW cohort exhibited excessive fighting and three mice progressively sustained severe injuries and were excluded from some tests. Other exclusions are listed in the figure legends.

Maternal separation procedures

In the maternal control (MC) group, with the exception of routine bi-weekly cage changes, pups were left untouched with the dams until weaning at P21. For both maternal separation procedures, dams were moved from home cages to a clean cage with ad libitum food and water during separation. Additionally, cages containing the dams were placed in a separate room to prevent any potential communication among the dams and pups. At the end of the separation period, both pups and dams were returned to the home cage.

Maternal separation (MS)

In the maternal separation group (MS), pups were removed from their home cages from P2 to P14 for 3 hours daily at randomized time points during the light cycle [10,23]. From P2 to 7, pups were placed together on a heating pad maintained at 37℃ during the separation period. From P7 to 14, pups were placed onto separate containers individually without heating. Pups were weaned at P21.

Maternal separation with early weaning (MSEW)

MSEW procedure was conducted as described previously [12,25]. Briefly, pups were removed from their home cages from P2 to 5 for 4 hours per day at randomized time points during the light cycle and placed together on a heating pad maintained at 37℃. From P6 to 16, pups were removed from the dams for 8 hours daily and placed into separate containers individually without heating. Pups were weaned at P17.

Behavioral testing of offspring

At P60, male mice were subjected to behavioral tests in the order listed below. Behavioral tests were only conducted in males.

Open field

The open field test was conducted in an arena measuring 40 cm by 40 cm by 30 cm in height using an automated VersaMax Animal Activity Monitor and VersaMax software v.4.12-1AFE (AccuScan Instruments, Columbus, Ohio) under ~400 lux illumination during the light phase (1200~1900). To capture the general locomotor activity of mice under non-aversive condition, the open field test was also conducted during the dark phase (2000~0000) under low illumination (<10 lux). Mice were put onto the center of the arena for 60-min testing and baseline locomotor activity was automatically recorded. Locomotion was measured as total distance traveled and stereotypy was measured as the number of consecutive beam breaks (<1 second).

Light/dark box

A red acrylic enclosure was inserted to divide the open field chamber into equal light and dark compartments for the light/dark box test. The light (40×20×30 cm3, ~400 lux) and dark (40×20×30 cm3, ~60 lux) compartments were connected via an opening (5×5 cm2) allowing transitions between both compartments. During the 10-min test, the percent of time an animal spent in the light compartment and locomotor activity were recorded using the VersaMax software. In addition, the number of transitions between the compartments was scored manually.

Tail suspension

The tail suspension test was performed to measure depression-like behavior by using the Tail Suspension Test Cubicle, PHM-300 (Med Associates, USA). Each mouse was suspended to a hook that was 20 cm above the ground with an adhesive tape for 6 min. Two animals were tested at the same time and separated by partitions to avoid potential communication between each other. Their overall movement was recorded using Tail Suspension Software, v.3.4.1.0 (Med Associates, Georgia, Vermont, USA) and activity thresholds were adjusted to detect immobility.

Forced swim test

The forced swim test was conducted using methods previously described [26]. Each mouse was gently placed in a custom-made cylinder (height, 40 cm; diameter, 20 cm) containing 13 cm of water at room temperature for 6 min and then returned to the home cage. Twenty-four hours later, the mouse was retested for 6 min. A professional video camcorder (Canon, Japan) was used to record trials for further analysis. Immobility time was scored using the Topscan Realtime Option software, v3.00 (Clever Sys, Inc., USA). A mouse was judged immobile when it ceased any movements except those that were necessary to keep its head above water. Immobility time is expressed as a percentage of the total time measurement.

Contextual and cued fear conditioning

Fear conditioning was conducted using the Fear Conditioning System, MED-VFC2-NIR-M (MedAssociates, USA). Mice were habituated to the chamber for two minutes. For the fear conditioning procedure, a 30-s tone (CS: 90 dB 10 kHz white noise or sine wave) was presented that co-terminated with an electric foot shock (US: 2 s, 1.0 mA). The conditioning procedure was repeated twice with inter-tone intervals of 2 min. Animals were returned to their home cages about 1 min after the last foot shock. 24 hours later, mice were tested for contextual and cued fear. For contextual fear memory recall, mice were replaced in the conditioning chamber for 5 min without any tone or foot shock, and then removed afterwards. To test cued fear memory, mice were placed into the original chamber that was redecorated to alter the visual cues, illumination and texture. The grills on the floor were covered with a white board, a red filter was placed over the overhead lamp, and the plain walls were covered with wallpaper of geometric patterns. Two min later, the CS was presented for 2 minutes. Mice were returned to home cages 1 min after termination of tone presentation.

In the fear conditioning experiments, the activity of animals was measured using Video Freeze Software, v 1.20.5.0 (MedAssociates, USA). Freezing responses to the conditioned context and tones were analyzed and expressed as total number of episodes and percentage of the observation time (freezing [%]=freezing time/observation time×100 %).

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed and plotted using GraphPad Prism (Graphpad Software, San Diego, California, USA) and presented as mean±SEM. Unless otherwise stated, data were analyzed by unpaired t-test. Measures on tail suspension, forced swim and open field tests across time was performed by Two-Way ANOVA with repeated measures followed by Tukey's honest significance test. A p<0.05 was accepted as statistically significant. In addition, extreme values were subjected to Grubb's test for outliers (http:// graphpad.com/quickcalcs/Grubbs1.cfm) and excluded from analysis.

MS and MSEW effects on depressive-like behaviors

Tail Suspension

Either MS or MSEW was carried out on separate batches of animals, with separate groups of control mice (MC) from littermate dams. There was no significant effect of either MS or MSEW paradigm on the stationary time in the tail suspension test (Fig. 1A and B). A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures showed no significant interaction between treatment conditions and time upon either MS (F(5, 65)=0.412, p=0.839) or MSEW (F(5, 75)=1.916, p=0.102). There was no main effect of treatment condition, given that the stationary time was not significantly different between MS and MC mice (F(1, 13)=0.507, p=0.489), and between MSEW and controls (F(1, 15)=2.574, p=0.130).

Forced swim test

Either MS or MSEW paradigm did not induce any significant effect on immobility time during the test trial of the forced swim test (Fig. 1C and D). A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures revealed no interaction between treatment condition and time for the MS (F(5, 84)=1.134, p=0.349) as well as MSEW (F(5,102)=1.208, p=0.311) cohorts. The main effects of treatment condition for both MS (F(1, 84)=0.469, p=0.495) and MSEW (F(1,102)=0.125, p=0.725) were not statistically significant. Taken together, these data indicated that either MS or MSEW paradigm did not produce any salient effects on the depressive-like behavior.

MS and MSEW effects on general locomotor activity and anxiety-like behavior

We tested animals in the open field for 60 minutes to measure the general locomotor activity. Distance traveled in the open field was defined as a general measure for horizontal activity. Time spent in the center zone was also measured as an index of anxiety-like behavior.

Open field (light phase)

During the open field exposure during light phase (approximately 400 lux), MS and MSEW conditions produced no difference in the distance traveled (Fig. 2A and C). A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures indicated no interaction between MS and time (F(11,231)=1.81, p=0.0528) and between MSEW and time (F(11,187)=0.591, p=0.835). Either MS or MSEW did not produce any effect on the total distance traveled (F(1, 21)=0.0515, p=0.823) and (F(1, 17)=0.312, p=0.584), respectively. With regards to anxiety-like behavior, MS and MSEW conditions had no significant effect with respect to the center duration in the open field (Fig. 2B and D). T-tests were performed on the time spent in the center and revealed no significant difference between MS (p=0.511), MSEW (p=0.488) compared to MC groups.

Open field (dark phase)

Interestingly, either MS or MSEW resulted in significant effects on general locomotor activity in an one-hour open-field test. Statistical analyses indicated no interaction between MS and time (F(11,154)=0.382, p=0.961) and between MSEW and time (F(11,198)=0.778, p=0.662), but there was a main effect of treatment condition; MS treated mice (F(1, 14)=12.4, p=0.0034) traveled more distance over time, while MSEW treated mice (F(1, 18)=4.94, p=0.0393) traveled less (Fig. 2E and G). A post-hoc analysis indicated that MS mice traveled significantly more than the MC group during the 30 (p=0.000517), 35 (p=0.00111), 40 (p=0.000796), and 60 minutes (p=0.00028) during the open-field test, indicating slight hyperactivity of animals. With regards to anxiety-like behavior, MS and MSEW conditions had no significant effect with respect to the center duration in the open field (Fig. 2F and H). T-tests indicated no difference in the time spent in the center for MS (p=0.517) and MSEW (p=0.360) compared to their respective control groups.

Light/dark box

MS and MSEW conditions did not elicit any significant effect on the time spent in either the light or dark compartments (Fig. 2I and K). A multiple t-test indicated no significant difference between the time spent in either the light or dark compartments for MS (p=0.721) or MSEW (p=0.318) mice, compared to the MC groups during the first 10 minutes. There was no significant difference in the number of transitions between MS (p=0.845) and MSEW (p=0.222), compared to MC groups (Fig. 2J and L). This indicates that both batches of MS and MSEW mice did not exhibit an increased anxiety-like phenotype in the light/dark box test.

MS and MSEW effects on acquisition and retention of fear memory

Either MS or MSEW condition did not produce any significant effects on the percentage of time spent freezing during the first day of a training phase in the fear-conditioning assay (Fig. 3A and B). Freezing behavior is the absence of any movement except for respiration and is used as an index for fear conditioning. A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures indicated no interaction between MS and trials (F(5, 75)=0.307, p=0.907) and between MSEW and trials (F(5, 80)=0.451, p=0.811). MS (F(1, 15)=0.0830, p=0.777) and MSEW (F(1, 16)=0.153, p=0.701) produced no effect on the percentage freezing time. This indicates that MS and MSEW mice were not impaired in acquiring emotional memories compared to the MC groups.

On the Day 2 of the fear-conditioning assay, MS and MSEW also did not produce significant effect on the percentage freezing time during the contextual and cued recall sessions. During the contextual recall session, there was no significant difference in the percentage freezing time between MS (p=0.169), compared to MC group, and MSEW (p=0.847), compared to MC group (Fig. 3C and D). In addition, no significant difference was found in the percentage freezing time between MS (p=0.888) and MC group and between MSEW (p=0.827) and MC group during the cued recall session (Fig. 3E and F). These data suggested that mice which experienced either MS or MSEW did not exhibit any significant impairment in fear memory consolidation and retention.

We sought to identify an early-life stress paradigm that could exert long-lasting and reproducible changes in animal behavior and stress reactivity to investigate a potential environmental trigger in the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders such as anxiety, depression, and schizophrenia [27,28]. The conventional 3-hour, MS paradigm was compared with a relatively new MSEW paradigm to determine which paradigm could be more effective in altering emotionality and increasing stress reactivity in adulthood. To our surprise, however, we observed that both MS and MSEW paradigms did not elicit any significant behavioral changes in depressive-like behavior and anxiety-like behavior (Fig. 1 and 2) as well as fear memory acquisition or recall in both the contextual and cue fear test (Fig. 3). The only difference we could observe was that mice which underwent MS paradigm traveled significantly further distance than controls, while mice which underwent MSEW traveled significantly less in the open field test, both of which were not consistent with outcomes from previous studies [10,21]. Our lack of reproducibility in eliciting previously reported behavioral change in depressive- and anxiety-like behaviors upon MS are surprisingly consistent with previous reviews [10,11]. More intriguingly, the MSEW paradigm, which was reported to produce robust anxiety-like behavior, did not elicit any significant behavioral change in C57BL/6J male mice [12,25].

One caveat is that the limited sample size of our groups may have resulted in insufficient power to detect significant effects [29]. In addition, factors such as the housing/experimental conditions and colony/sub-strain divergence may explain why our results differ from similar studies conducted with C57/BL6 mice [30,31,32] . Further studies are needed to assess if these results are replicable.

These lack of consistent phenotypes upon MS and MSEW paradigms may indicate the limited effectiveness of these paradigms in modeling an early life stress in mice, compared to other species such as rats or primates [33,34]. Instead, although we did not investigate in this study, early-life stress paradigms based on stressing dam and on additional early life stressors could be more effective. These paradigms are: reduced nesting material from P2-P9, which disrupts dam-pup interactions [35], social isolation of young animals following MS [36], and maternal stress in the form of tube restraints or forced swimming on dams [37]. Rather than focusing on modifying the duration of maternal separation, alterations to the early rearing environment or additional stressors may be a better alternative or inclusion in maternal separation paradigms.

Taken together, we observed no significant behavioral change upon either MS or MSEW paradigm on male mice. MSEW, which has been gaining popularity over MS recently, could not elicit previously reported results. Our results necessitate further replication studies and the optimization of a stress paradigm in mice to bring about long-lasting and reproducible behavioral changes. The development of such paradigms will be critical in investigating the causal relationship between early-life aversive experience and the pathophysiology of human psychiatric disorders.

Fig. 1. MS and MSEW mice did not exhibit depression-related behavior compared to controls. There was no significant difference in the time spent stationary on the tail suspension test between (A) MS mice and MC mice and (B) MSEW and MC mice. One mouse from each group in the MS cohort was excluded for excessive climbing and three mice from the MC group in the MSEW cohort were excluded for severe injuries. There was no significant difference in the immobility time during the second trial of a forced swim test between (C) MS and MC mice and (D) MSEW and MC mice. One mouse in the MS cohort was excluded for excessive hyperactivity and two mice in the MC group in the MSEW cohort were excluded for severe injuries. All values are means±SEM.
Fig. 2. MS and MSEW mice show changes in locomotor activity in the open field during the dark phase but did not display anxiety-like behavior compared to controls. In the open field conducted during the light phase, there were no significant differences on distance traveled over time between (A) MS and MC group and (C) MSEW and MC group. On the time spent in the center zone, which can be interpreted as an anxiety-related behavior, there were no significant differences between (B) MS and MC group and (D) MSEW and MC group. Two mice from the MC group in the MSEW cohort were excluded for severe injuries. In the open field conducted during the dark phase (E) MS mice traveled significantly more than controls during the 30, 35, 40 and 60 min (p<0.01) and (G) MSEW traveled significantly less over time compared to MC (p<0.05), but post-hoc tests were not significant. There were no differences in the time spent in center of the open field between (F) MS and MC and (H) MSEW and MC mice during the dark phase. One mouse from the MC group in the MS cohort was excluded for as an outlier for excessive hyperactivity and one mouse from the MC group in the MSEW cohort was excluded for severe injuries. In the light/dark box test, there were no significant differences in the time spent in light and dark compartments as well as the number of transitions between both compartments between (I and K) MS and MC mice and (J and L) MSEW and MC mice. *Indicates p<0.01 for the post-hoc test for that particular time bin. All values are means±SEM.
Fig. 3. Mice which experienced MS or MSEW did not exhibit impairments in hippocampal and amygdala-dependent fear-related emotional memories. In the acquisition phase of the fear conditioning test, there were no significant difference between (A) MS and MC mice and (B) MSEW and MC mice in the percent freezing time when exposed to the conditioned stimulus (tone) paired with the unconditioned stimulus (foot shock). Habit, Habituation; US, Unconditioned Stimulus; CS, Conditioned Stimulus. Three mice from the MC group in the MSEW cohort were excluded for severe injuries. During the contextual fear recall 24 hours after training, there were no significant differences between (C) MS and MC mice and (D) MSEW and MC mice on percent freezing time. During the cue fear recall 24 hours after training, there was also no significant differences between (E) MS and MC mice and (F) MSEW and MC mice on percent freezing time. One mice from MS and MSEW group were each excluded as outliers for low freezing responses. All values are means±SEM.
Table. 1. Summary of some commonly used maternal separation paradigms and behavioral effects in adult male mice on emotion-related behaviors
StrainsMaternal separation paradigmBehavioral phenotype in MS group in adulthood (compared to controls)Reference
C57BL/6J3 hours MS on P1–P9Elevated anxiety-like behavior in EPM and OF.[14]
CD-13 hours MS on P2–P14Decreased anxiety-like behavior in EPM.[15]
CD-124 hour MS on P12No changes in locomotor activity in OF. No change in total time spent immobile in FST.[16]
C57BLl/6NCrlBR3 hours MS on P0–P9No changes in anxiety-related behavior in DWT.[17]
C57BL/6J3 hours MS on P1–P10No change in DWT. Decreased anxiety-like behavior in dark phase EZM but increased anxiety in EPM.[18]
129S1, BALB/cByJ, C57BL/6J, DBA/2J, FVB/NJ3 hours MS on P0–13No overall changes in anxiety and depressive-related behavior in EPM, LDB, OF and FST.[10]
C57BL/6J24 hours MS on P9No change in OF activity. Decreased anxiety in EPM.[19]
C57BL/6J3 hour MS on P1–14Increased anxiety in EPM but no change in anxiety behavior in OF.[20]
C57BL/6J and BALB/c3 hours MS on P1–14No change in depressive-like behavior in FST.[21]
C57BL/6J and DBA/2J24 hour MS on P9Lowered anxiety-related behavior in OF. No changes in hole board exploratory activity. No change in depressive-like behavior in FST.[22]
C57BL/6J3 hours MS on P2–14No change in LDB anxiety-related behavior. NSF behavior not affected. Decreased anxiety-like behavior in OF in last 15 minutes. No change in depressive-like behavior in FST.[23]
CCR7 deficient C57BL/6J3 hours MS on P1–14No change in anxiety-like behavior in OF and EZM. No change in depressive-like behavior in saccharin preference test.[24]

MS, maternal separation; EPM, Elevated Plus Maze; EZM, Elevated Zero Maze; FST, Forced Swim Test; OF, Open field; DWT, Defensive withdrawal test; LDB, Light/dark box; NSF, Novelty suppressed feeding.


  1. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, Dube SR, Giles WH. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006;256:174-186.
    Pubmed
  2. Fryers T, Brugha T. Childhood determinants of adult psychiatric disorder. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2013;9:1-50.
    Pubmed
  3. Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B. Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Med 1997;27:951-959.
    Pubmed
  4. Mathew SJ, Mao X, Coplan JD, Smith EL, Sackeim HA, Gorman JM, Shungu DC. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortical pathology in generalized anxiety disorder: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:1119-1121.
    Pubmed
  5. Malter Cohen M, Jing D, Yang RR, Tottenham N, Lee FS, Casey BJ. Early-life stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2013;110:18274-18278.
    Pubmed
  6. Levine S. A further study of infantile handling and adult avoidance learning. J Pers 1956;25:70-80.
    Pubmed
  7. Hofer MA. Studies on how early maternal separation produces behavioral change in young rats. Psychosom Med 1975;37:245-264.
    Pubmed
  8. Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Nemeroff CB. Parental depression: animal models of an adverse life event. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:1265-1283.
    Pubmed
  9. Pryce CR, Bettschen D, Nanz-Bahr NI, Feldon J. Comparison of the effects of early handling and early deprivation on conditioned stimulus, context, and spatial learning and memory in adult rats. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:883-893.
    Pubmed
  10. Millstein RA, Holmes A. Effects of repeated maternal separation on anxiety- and depression-related phenotypes in different mouse strains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2007;31:3-17.
    Pubmed
  11. Tractenberg SG, Levandowski ML, de Azeredo LA, Orso R, Roithmann LG, Hoffmann ES, Brenhouse H, Grassi-Oliveira R. An overview of maternal separation effects on behavioural outcomes in mice: evidence from a four-stage methodological systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016;68:489-503.
    Pubmed
  12. George ED, Bordner KA, Elwafi HM, Simen AA. Maternal separation with early weaning: a novel mouse model of early life neglect. BMC Neurosci 2010;11:123.
    Pubmed
  13. Gracia-Rubio I, Moscoso-Castro M, Pozo OJ, Marcos J, Nadal R, Valverde O. Maternal separation induces neuroinflammation and long-lasting emotional alterations in mice. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2016;65:104-117.
    Pubmed
  14. Romeo RD, Mueller A, Sisti HM, Ogawa S, McEwen BS, Brake WG. Anxiety and fear behaviors in adult male and female C57BL/6 mice are modulated by maternal separation. Horm Behav 2003;43:561-567.
    Pubmed
  15. Venerosi A, Cirulli F, Capone F, Alleva E. Prolonged perinatal AZT administration and early maternal separation: effects on social and emotional behaviour of periadolescent mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;74:671-681.
    Pubmed
  16. Macrì S, Laviola G. Single episode of maternal deprivation and adult depressive profile in mice: interaction with cannabinoid exposure during adolescence. Behav Brain Res 2004;154:231-238.
    Pubmed
  17. Parfitt DB, Levin JK, Saltstein KP, Klayman AS, Greer LM, Helmreich DL. Differential early rearing environments can accentuate or attenuate the responses to stress in male C57BL/6 mice. Brain Res 2004;1016:111-118.
    Pubmed
  18. Parfitt DB, Walton JR, Corriveau EA, Helmreich DL. Early life stress effects on adult stress-induced corticosterone secretion and anxiety-like behavior in the C57BL/6 mouse are not as robust as initially thought. Horm Behav 2007;52:417-426.
    Pubmed
  19. Fabricius K, Wörtwein G, Pakkenberg B. The impact of maternal separation on adult mouse behaviour and on the total neuron number in the mouse hippocampus. Brain Struct Funct 2008;212:403-416.
    Pubmed
  20. van Heerden JH, Russell V, Korff A, Stein DJ, Illing N. Evaluating the behavioural consequences of early maternal separation in adult C57BL/6 mice; the importance of time. Behav Brain Res 2010;207:332-342.
    Pubmed
  21. Savignac HM, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. Resistance to early-life stress in mice: effects of genetic background and stress duration. Front Behav Neurosci 2011;5:13.
    Pubmed
  22. Kember RL, Dempster EL, Lee TH, Schalkwyk LC, Mill J, Fernandes C. Maternal separation is associated with strain-specific responses to stress and epigenetic alterations to Nr3c1, Avp, and Nr4a1 in mouse. Brain Behav 2012;2:455-467.
    Pubmed
  23. Own LS, Patel PD. Maternal behavior and offspring resiliency to maternal separation in C57Bl/6 mice. Horm Behav 2013;63:411-417.
    Pubmed
  24. Harrison EL, Jaehne EJ, Jawahar MC, Corrigan F, Baune BT. Maternal separation modifies behavioural and neuroendocrine responses to stress in CCR7 deficient mice. Behav Brain Res 2014;263:169-175.
    Pubmed
  25. Carlyle BC, Duque A, Kitchen RR, Bordner KA, Coman D, Doolittle E, Papademetris X, Hyder F, Taylor JR, Simen AA. Maternal separation with early weaning: a rodent model providing novel insights into neglect associated developmental deficits. Dev Psychopathol 2012;24:1401-1416.
    Pubmed
  26. Porsolt RD, Le Pichon M, Jalfre M. Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Nature 1977;266:730-732.
    Pubmed
  27. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, Mc-Carthy MM, Nemeroff CB, Reyes TM, Simerly RB, Susser ES, Nestler EJ. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2010;68:314-319.
    Pubmed
  28. Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA. Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;214:55-70.
    Pubmed
  29. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, Munafò MR. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013;14:365-376.
    Pubmed
  30. Mitchell E, Klein SL, Argyropoulos KV, Sharma A, Chan RB, Toth JG, Barboza L, Bavley C, Bortolozzi A, Chen Q, Liu B, Ingenito J, Mark W, Dudakov J, Gross S, Di Paolo G, Artigas F, van den, Toth M. Behavioural traits propagate across generations via segregated iterative-somatic and gametic epigenetic mechanisms. Nat Commun 2016;7:11492.
    Pubmed
  31. Juetten J, Einat H. Behavioral differences in black Swiss mice from separate colonies: implications for modeling domains of mania. Behav Pharmacol 2012;23:211-214.
    Pubmed
  32. Bryant CD, Zhang NN, Sokoloff G, Fanselow MS, Ennes HS, Palmer AA, McRoberts JA. Behavioral differences among C57BL/6 substrains: implications for transgenic and knockout studies. J Neurogenet 2008;22:315-331.
    Pubmed
  33. Holmes A, le Guisquet AM, Vogel E, Millstein RA, Leman S, Belzung C. Early life genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors shaping emotionality in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:1335-1346.
    Pubmed
  34. Pryce CR, Rüedi-Bettschen D, Dettling AC, Feldon J. Early life stress: long-term physiological impact in rodents and primates. News Physiol Sci 2002;17:150-155.
    Pubmed
  35. Rice CJ, Sandman CA, Lenjavi MR, Baram TZ. A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology 2008;149:4892-4900.
    Pubmed
  36. Niwa M, Matsumoto Y, Mouri A, Ozaki N, Nabeshima T. Vulnerability in early life to changes in the rearing environment plays a crucial role in the aetiopathology of psychiatric disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011;14:459-477.
    Pubmed
  37. Weiss IC, Franklin TB, Vizi S, Mansuy IM. Inheritable effect of unpredictable maternal separation on behavioral responses in mice. Front Behav Neurosci 2011;5:3.
    Pubmed