Articles

  • the Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences

Article

Review Article

Exp Neurobiol 2019; 28(1): 1-16

Published online February 28, 2019

https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2019.28.1.1

© The Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences

Cognitive Enhancement in Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders Using Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS): A Review of Modalities, Potential Mechanisms and Future Implications

Tammy D. Kim1,2,†, Gahae Hong2,†, Jungyoon Kim1,2, and Sujung Yoon1,2*

1Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Ewha Womans University, Seoul 03760, Korea.

2Ewha Brain Institute, Ewha Womans University, Seoul 03760, Korea.

Correspondence to: *To whom correspondence should be addressed.
TEL: 82 2-3277-2478, FAX: 82 2-3277-6562
e-mail: sujungjyoon@ewha.ac.kr
These authors contributed equally to this work.

Received: December 12, 2018; Revised: February 7, 2019; Accepted: February 15, 2019

Cognitive enhancement refers to the improvement of cognitive function related to deficits that occurred as part of a certain illness. However, the term cognitive enhancement does not yet have a definitive meaning, and its connotations often vary depending on the research of interest. Recently, research interests are growing towards enhancing human cognition beyond what has traditionally been considered necessary using various brain devices. The phenomenon of exceeding the cognitive abilities of individuals who are already functional has also introduced new terminologies as means to classify between cognitive enhancing procedures that are part of treatment versus simply supplementary. Of the many devices used to attain cognitive enhancement, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a unique neurostimulatory device that has demonstrated significant improvements in various cognitive domains including memory and cognitive processing skills. While many studies have supported the safety and efficacy of TMS in treatment, there has yet to be an optimization in parameter for TMS that is catered to a certain target group. The current paper aims to review with perspective the many studies that have used TMS for the purpose of cognitive enhancement and provide further insight on the development of an optimal stimulation parameter. The paper reviews 41 peer-reviewed articles that used TMS for cognitive enhancement, summarizes the findings that were apparent for each distinct parameter, and discusses future directions regarding TMS as an elective tool for healthy individuals while considering some of the ethical perspectives that may be warranted.


Keywords: Transcranial magnetic stimulation, Cognition, Noninvasive, Memory, Neuropsychological tests

The emergence of new and innovative technological tools has become a growing trend in the field of neuroscience, as they provide much insight regarding our knowledge of the brain's capacity [1]. Present day research emphasizes the role of neurological tools as treatment strategies or parts of, such as preliminary screening tools for the future onset of various diseases and disorders [2,3]. Of the many neurological devices, those that have the ability to improve cognitive function have become particularly of interest, as investigating new ways of enhancing human abilities is an ongoing collective goal in neuroscience [4]. Abundant research within the field of neuroscience demonstrate the diverse applicability of neurological devices in enhancing cognitive function, including various diseases and impairments [5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14].

Cognitive enhancement, clinically speaking, refers to intervention strategies that aim to improve cognitive function that has shown deficit as part of a certain illness [15]. However, as the topic of interest in neuroscience research slowly progressed towards enhancing human cognition beyond what is traditionally considered necessary, newer definitions of cognitive enhancement emerged as means to segregate between cognitive enhancing procedures that are treatment-driven versus supplementary [16,17,18]. There are various approaches to achieve cognitive enhancement such as in forms of therapy [19], pharmacological treatment [20], and the use of a technological device [21]. Among these, noninvasive brain stimulation (NBS) is a technique which emerged in the late 1990s that alters brain activity by changing neural function via intervention [22], and has received much attention in research due to having less generalized side effects. In particular, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is one of the most utilized methods for the purpose of altering cognitive function. TMS refers to the stimulation of the nerve generated by electrically induced magnetic fields, and is unique and comparable to other NBS technologies due to its neurostimulatory nature [23,24].

TMS has become popular as it yielded significant findings in the improvement of cognitive function domains [25,26]. Previous studies have shown that healthy individuals who have undergone TMS treatment demonstrated increased episodic memory [27], working memory [28], and motor learning performance [29,30,31]. Findings of earlier TMS research also helped elucidate the capability that brain regions can be manipulated to yield altered cognitive function [32]. Recent studies have also introduced the combined application of TMS and neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) as well as potential biomarkers for cognitive function [33]. This collective approach of various neuroimaging techniques have enabled us to further understand ways to enhance cognition, manipulate the human brain and categorize by regions of activation in respect to specific cognitive domains.

The purpose of this article is to critically review the usage of TMS in humans for the purpose of cognitive enhancement. The review is divided into sections as follows: a methodology of the data selection process conducted, an introduction of the basic mechanisms of TMS along with the different types of TMS used in clinical trials for cognitive enhancement, a summary of the results from modern TMS studies and their implications, a discussion to note the possible ways of improvement in the current methodologies for cognitive enhancement using TMS, and concluding with the possible ethical concerns that may follow in the usage of TMS for cognitive enhancement. The paper will focus on reviewing open-label trials and randomized controlled trials that have been performed within recent years, in order to discuss the more longterm effects of TMS treatment, as opposed to the immediate online effects of noninvasive brain stimulation.

This article provides a review of the TMS studies (1999~2018) that primarily focus on the stimulation of the prefrontal cortex, an area of the brain which has reliably shown to influence cognitive function [34]. The current review with perspective is based on a broad spectrum of samples including patients suffering from psychiatric or neurological diseases and healthy volunteers for the purpose of treating cognitive dysfunction and cognitive enhancement, respectively. By performing a literature search in the following databases: PubMed (1999~2018) and MEDLINE (1999~2018), 41 articles were identified and chosen upon a careful review of the content of the articles and were deemed as relative to the current review. The identified articles consist of 10 open-label trials and 31 randomized controlled trials - including within-subject, shamcontrolled or crossover designs. Of these, 22 comprised of trials with depression or PTSD, 6 with schizophrenia, 4 with impairment in the cognitive domains, 3 with neurological conditions, 1 with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), and 5 studies involving healthy participants only.

Basic mechanism of TMS and emerging trend

The stimulation from TMS is a stored charge that is delivered outside the scalp. The magnetic field generated by the TMS penetrates the skull, and electrical current is generated in the cortical neurons that are located in the area under where the TMS coil is placed [35]. This current then depolarizes the neuronal membranes by mimicking electrical stimulation, thus triggering action potential. Unlike neuromodulatory devices which have low temporal resolution despite similarity in spatial resolution as the TMS, TMS has been noted as a neurostimulatory device as a result of its relatively high temporal resolution [36]. The electrical current from TMS has shown to induce increase in action potentials that are sufficient enough to yield a major mechanic difference [35], suggesting a more long-term alteration in cognitive performance when used for the purpose of cognitive enhancement [32].

While the general mechanism of TMS has been established over the years as mentioned above, the optimal method and parameters to applying TMS for the purpose of cognitive enhancement has been long debated. During the earlier years of research in cognitive enhancement, TMS was more typically applied to stimulate the peripheral nerves [37]. However, over the last few decades, transcranial magnetic stimulation of the cerebral cortex has received much attention as it turns out the application of TMS over the motor cortex can directly stimulate the corticospinal axon [35]. Furthermore, the application of TMS over non-motor areas of the brain also became of interest as it extended possible research applications with respect to brain-behavior relationships [36]. With direct cortical stimulation, in contrast to peripheral nerve stimulation, both excitatory and inhibitory effects in cognitive function were observed depending on the frequency of the stimulation. In addition, as newer technologies emerged in the field of neuroimaging such as fMRI and neuronavigation, researchers are now able to observe the direct neural effects of TMS by working in tandem with such methodologies, potentially increasing the reliability of these studies [38].

Repetitive TMS

Throughout the years, research also demonstrated that each type of TMS application elicits distinct physiological results. Among these, repetitive TMS (rTMS) has now become the most utilized methodology, due to its ability to alter cortical inhibition and excitation according to stimulation frequency [26]. In particular, recent research emphasize the application of rTMS for the purpose of cognitive enhancement. rTMS applies a certain number of stimulations per session to the area of stimulation over a prolonged period of time - typically between 20 to 40 minutes of treatment time - and its large number of condensed stimulation is what enables alterations in task performance. In the past, fast and repetitive exposure of rTMS was believed to have detrimental negative effects on cognitive processes, due to its disruptive nature [26]. However, studies later clarified that this is only in the case when rTMS is applied while simultaneously performing an online task. On the contrary, studies have indicated that rTMS that is delivered prior to a task or in short periods in between tasks can enhance the overall cognitive task performance [32], and therefore became popularized in the field of cognitive enhancement. Of the many studies that focus on the cognitive enhancing effects of TMS as discussed in the current review, the majority have used rTMS as the primary method of treatment. rTMS may therefore be a promising method of TMS treatment for various deficits and further improvements related to cognition, in both populations of cognitive deficits such as those with ADHD or dementia, as well as healthy populations.

One of the conventional ways to categorize rTMS is by frequency, which is typically labelled as low or high frequency. Low frequency rTMS (LF-rTMS) refers to a frequency of 1~4 Hz and is known to induce transient suppression or cortical excitability by depolarizing neurons. Studies that utilized LF-rTMS have shown that it indirectly affects emotion and behavior [38], and some improvement in language related tasks [39]. However, there are very few studies that have shown cognitive enhancement using LF-rTMS and as such, high frequency rTMS (HF-rTMS) is the most widely used technique for cognitive enhancement in clinical trials as well as healthy individuals.

HF-rTMS is defined as frequencies that are 5 Hz or higher, and has been found to enhance long-term cortical excitability which sustains up to a month subsequent to the treatment [40]. HF-rTMS was initially thought to have detrimental negative effects on cognitive processes due to its disruptive nature and high magnetic field intensity. However, studies later clarified that HF-rTMS that is applied prior to a task transiently enhances cortical excitability in a more effective manner as compared with LF-rTMS, and have further proven that it has more long-term effects in the cognitive domains than other types of TMS [25].

Brain regions of interest for TMS

One of the factors to note with respect to TMS is its location-specific nature. The neurobehavioral effects from TMS can vary tremendously depending on the particular brain region that is stimulated. Therefore, noting the location of stimulation prior to treatment is essential depending on the treatment group, especially so in the clinical field. For instance, in the case of treatment for motor function in patients with Parkinson's disease, stimulation would more likely be applied to the primary motor cortex in order to treat for the deficits in motor function [41]. In contrast, in the case of patients with psychotic disorders including depression and OCD, stimulation would be applied within the vicinity of the prefrontal cortex to treat for psychological symptoms [42].

One of the key anatomical regions of interest in cognitive enhancement via TMS is the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). The DLPFC is part of the Brodmann area 9 and is significantly involved in human cognition, particularly in various processing skills including working memory, attentional control, and processing episodic information [34,43]. Earlier fMRI studies have shown that the left DLPFC is activated during times of task performance, and its activation is increased in correlation with the difficulty of the task [34]. Since the discovery of the function of the DLPFC, many studies have undergone experimental studies to validate both the safety and efficacy as well as the cognitive enhancing effects of TMS to the left DLPFC. To name a few, numerous randomized, sham-controlled studies have found that applying rTMS to the left DLPFC enhances response inhibition [44,45,46,47], working memory [48,49], delayed improvement in logical memory [50] and verbal fluency [51]. Research have also diversified in methodology, and found that the simultaneous application of rTMS to both hemispheres of the DLPFC improved episodic memory, a finding which revealed that the underlying mechanism involves the alteration in frontal gamma oscillatory activity [52]. While there are several selective studies that found improvement in the cognitive domain by applying TMS elsewhere other than the DLPFC such as the left primary motor cortex to improve associate memory [53], most studies have found that stimulating the left DLPFC via rTMS yields the most effective cognitive enhancement.

Within recent years, numerous studies have been done as means to validate the safety and cognitive enhancement effects of rTMS treatment. Many of these studies have focused on populations that typically show cognitive deficits as part of a prolonged illness, including depression, schizophrenia, mild cognitive impairment and dementia, and attention deficit hyperactive disorder. Some studies also began to focus on the cognitive enhancement of healthy individuals with normal cognitive abilities for improvement purposes [23,31,45,46,49].

While these abovementioned studies have explored various cognitive domains using a large spectrum of cognitive rating, the primary cognitive domain that are of interest may be classified into two groups - memory and processing skills. Memory and processing skills are classic examples of the broadly used term ‘intellect’ [54]. Having a large memory capacity and high processing skill are considered valuable intellectual traits among the healthy population, while the lack of brings increasing concern to the aging population and those of cognitive deficit. Here, we review some of the randomized controlled trials that have been conducted over the years using rTMS with the outcome variables of one or both of the abovementioned cognitive domains which are memory and processing skills. We also suggest possible neural pathways of rTMS treatment that may explain for its high efficacy in cognitive enhancement by referring to previous findings regarding brain networks and connectivity. Furthermore, we categorize the outcomes of the studies reviewed according to various samples, including diseased and healthy populations.

Effects-based cognitive domains: memory

In the case of rTMS and cognitive enhancement, studies have primarily focused on the domains of memory that are short-term in that they are not permanent changes to one's cognitive function (Table 1). Among the various types of memory, those that fit into this description in previous studies have been working memory [6,8,9,48,49,51,56,57,58,87], episodic memory [27,51,59,60], and logical memory [27,55].

Working memory refers to a cognitive system where information is temporarily stored for the purpose of processing, including complex cognitive tasks such as language comprehension, learning, and reasoning [55]. Throughout the years, studies have indicated that certain diseases show cognitive deficits in the working memory spectrum, and therefore affect one's ability to store and manipulate information online [55]. Within the field of schizophrenia, meta-analyses were performed to confirm this cognitive deficit in working memory in schizophrenic patients [56], and other analyses have also been performed in the field of depression [8], as well as ADHD [6,9]. While the definitive ways to assess working memory has not yet been stratified and there is a large variety of assessments that can be used, most trial studies have used similar tests to assess working memory. Within the working memory spectrum, many studies have used tasks including digit span, spatial span, and n-back test to measure verbal working memory, as well as spatial span and spatial working memory tasks from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) for visual and spatial working memory. In the current review, 15 studies have been chosen collectively which have shown significant improvements in working memory using the above methods of assessment along with rTMS (Table 1). Among these, certain studies have suggested that the improvement in working memory in relation to HF-rTMS may yield greater effect when the working memory load is higher and be related to the reduction of excessive gamma oscillatory activity, as found in Barr and colleagues [57]. In addition, Levkovitz and colleagues (2011) have also noted that certain working memory domains may have greater effects depending on the area of the brain that is activated during the task performance, such as frontal lobe-related tasks in contrast to tasks that activate the striatal-parietal region of the brain [48]. While working memory can be further broken down into spatial working memory and verbal working memory, previous randomized controlled trials have shown significant improvements in both domains of working memory after the treatment of rTMS [58].

Although limited in number, several studies have also indicated improvements in the three pinnacle memory domains, which are episodic, semantic and autobiographical memory after TMS treatment. While these memory domains are primarily related to the medial temporal lobe and the hippocampus as suggested in previous studies [59], rTMS treatment to the DLPFC have yielded significant results. Episodic memory, in particular, have shown to be reduced in individuals with cognitive deficits such as those with amnestic mild cognitive impairment [60], and have been of interest to best prolong and retain in the general population. Gagnon and colleagues (2011) have shown that healthy individuals with normal cognitive abilities have shown increased episodic memory as measured via Remember/Know recognition task, although this was done so using a paired-pulse TMS methodology [23]. Furthermore, Fitzgerald and colleagues (2003) have shown improvements in semantic memory as well as autobiographical memory after the treatment of rTMS, where both low and high frequency rTMS applied to the left DLPFC for 4 consecutive weeks have yielded significant improvements [51].

Effects-based cognitive domains: processing skills

Another beneficial factor that has been suggested as a result of rTMS stimulation is the enhancement of cognitive processing. Cognitive processing is a domain of cognitive function that is distinct from memory, as it is less related to knowledge and more towards non-executive functioning such as selective attention. While the idea of enhancing one's cognitive abilities is generally familiarized as enhancement regarding intellect such as memory domains, processing skills are also a key factor that is significantly associated with intellect. Studies have shown that individuals with higher intelligence quotients tend to have faster processing speed [61], which may arguably be affected by the neural system. Therefore, the electrical current that stimulates the cortical neurons via rTMS may affect the neural pathways in such a way that it leads to a more efficient neural network in terms of processing information.

Within recent years, several studies have demonstrated the effects of rTMS in the improvement of cognitive processing (Table 2). In particular, many of these studies have used the Stroop interference task as the method of assessment for cognitive processing, as it has long been noted as a reliable way of measuring top-down attentional control including attentional vigilance and inhibition [31,44,45,46,47]. While these studies were based on many different target groups including healthy individuals, those with depression, and those with cognitive impairment, all the studies have shown consistency in their findings in that HF-rTMS to the left DLPFC increased the overall performance in the Stroop interference task. The collective findings suggest that the effects of rTMS do not necessarily differ between individuals based on intellect or illness, but rather largely depend on the application process of TMS including the area of stimulation and frequency.

In addition, there are many studies that support the effects of rTMS in enhanced cognitive processing using lesser conventional methods of assessment for cognitive processing including the assessment of psychomotor speed using the Motor Agitation and Retardation Scale [62], the Divided Attention Task [38], and the Letter Cancellation Test [47], all of which demonstrated a significant improvement in task performance.

Group-based cognitive outcome: healthy individuals

Previous studies on cognitive enhancement using rTMS can also be generalized into categories of sample groups. While the application of rTMS has been FDA-approved for the target population of individuals with depression, studies have also shown that healthy individuals show improved cognition after rTMS. In particular, Vanderhasselt and colleagues (2006; 2007) have shown consistent results of the effects of rTMS on the DLPFC in improved performance on the Stroop interference task. The two studies support the efficacy of rTMS in higher performance of attentional control for individuals without any particular neural deficit, and further suggest that the effects are significant regardless of the hemisphere of stimulation [45,46]. Additional studies demonstrated improved attention as a result of rTMS in healthy individuals, using other measures for attentional control including Conner's Continuous Performance Task [31].

Furthermore, numerous rTMS studies on healthy individuals also demonstrated various domains of memory enhancement, including recognition memory and working memory [27,49]. While rTMS on healthy individuals has not yet proven to improve long-term memory or autobiographical memory as found in a depression model [51], improved recognition memory within healthy individuals may support previous theory that recognition is largely affected by changes in the hippocampus of the brain [63,64]. Given that the prefrontal cortex is stimulated in rTMS treatment, coupled with the fact that the prefrontal cortex interplays with the hippocampus in human memory [65], rTMS treatment to the DLPFC may indirectly affect the hippocampus in such a way that enhances recognition memory of healthy individuals. Future studies that investigate the connectivity and activation of brain networks that include the area of stimulation in rTMS may contribute to the delineation of the neural pathways of rTMS treatment.

Group-based cognitive outcome: depression and PTSD

In contrast to healthy individuals with a normal range of cognitive function, there is a much higher number of studies that demonstrate the efficacy of rTMS in individuals with disorders including depression and PTSD (Table 1 and 2). While many studies have reported cognitive deficit as a secondary symptom of depression and/or related emotional distress including PTSD, the precise mechanism that underlie this effect is yet to be elucidated. One of the theories behind this effect that is most agreed upon is the underactivation of the prefrontal cortex of the brain as a response to cope for the psychological distress [66]. The underactivation is coupled with lower cerebral blood flow to the prefrontal cortex, which results in the disturbance of its key function which is higher thinking and executive functioning. The application of rTMS to the left DLPFC may act as an enhancer to the deficit of the prefrontal areas including activation and cerebral blood flow, and has shown to improve memories of those under depression and PTSD in various domains including episodic memory, spatial memory, verbal fluency, and executive function [51,67,68,69,70]. rTMS treatment in depression has also demonstrated improved attentional control including the trail-making test and Stroop interference task [7,45,46,67,70,71,72,73,74]. While cognitive enhancement through rTMS in models of depression and PTSD has been consistent, further neuroimaging studies that investigate the physiological changes of the brain in response to rTMS may benefit in the understanding of the precise neural mechanisms underlying the magnetic stimulation of the DLPFC.

Potential neurological pathway underlying the effects of rTMS

The current review demonstrates that rTMS treatment has a significant effect in cognitive function across various groups including healthy and diseased populations. However, it is noteworthy that the precise neurological pathway that underlies the effects of rTMS has yet to be elucidated. While the associations between brain regions and their functional roles have been investigated throughout the history of neuroscience, research that directly investigate the neurological pathways in the perspective of rTMS treatment is still limited. Further studies that directly observe the alterations in certain brain network connectivity related to the regions of stimulation in rTMS may help understand its mechanisms, along with the development of novel rTMS parameters. Previous studies in cognitive neuroscience have demonstrated that certain brain networks and their inter-connectivity with other regions of the brain strongly affect cognitive outcomes including memory. A recent study by Bridge and colleagues (2017) have shown the significant association between the hippocampus and the frontoparietal network (FPN) of the brain on retrieval memory [75]. This inter-connectivity relationship between brain regions and networks may help explain the neural pathways that underlie rTMS stimulation, as the DLPFC is one of the main components of the FPN. The magnetic stimulation to the DLPFC may strengthen the connectivity within the FPN, and consequently increase the inter-connectivity strength between the FPN and hippocampus, yielding an enhancement in memory. This particular mechanism may also be generalizable to other memory domains, and further studies that investigate this process according to different subtypes of memory may help elucidate the validity and generalizability of this potential pathway.

Furthermore, studies have also noted the significant role of the precuneus in cognitive function. The precuneus is another major component of the FPN alongside with the DLPFC, as well as a key modulator of the default mode network (DMN) of the brain [76]. By contributing to two of the major brain networks, the precuneus is involved with a number of important functions including performance of integrated tasks, eye movement, imagery, and self-processing operations [76]. Studies including Utevsky and colleagues (2014) have demonstrated the significance of the precuneus in modulating the default-mode network (DMN), particularly by the optimization or enhancement of the DMN [77]. This optimization of the DMN activity was also found to be in correlation with enhanced resting-state cognition, which can also be interchangeable to attentiveness [77]. Considering these findings, rTMS treatment to the DLPFC may stimulate and optimize the activity of the precuneus within the FPN, resulting in increased connectivity strength between the precuneus and the DMN. The heightened attentiveness that results from this potential neural pathway may explain for the increased processing speed as summarized in the current review (Table 2). A detailed schematic diagram of the two potential neural pathways that underlie as part of the mechanisms of rTMS is also provided (Fig. 1).

Despite these potential mechanisms of TMS, however, it is noteworthy that contradictory findings have also been present. A portion of the previous research that used similar if not identical parameters to those that demonstrated significant cognitive enhancement via rTMS treatment, also demonstrated insignificant findings (Table 3). As many of these studies used both parameters as well as outcome variables that are similar to those described in Tables 1 and 2, this suggests that the cognitive enhancing effects of rTMS should not yet be considered as valid.

The current review demonstrates that the development of neuroscientific tools such as TMS may be an innovative and reliable method of intervention for cognitive enhancement. However, as the application of TMS to healthy individuals with normal cognitive function becomes more prevalent, certain ethical questions arise regarding the accessibility and unknown risks of TMS. For instance, TMS is still under the developmental stages of parameter optimization, and adverse effects such as the compromise of tissue within the area of stimulation have not yet been discussed [78]. Although many experiments and patient lesion studies have supported the safety and efficacy of TMS, concerns remain due to a lack of long-term longitudinal studies that investigate the effects of TMS on a tissue-based level. These absences warrant for further ethical discussions which aim to analyze the effects of TMS in cognitive enhancement of healthy individuals who are neurally intact.

First of all, the use of TMS as cognitive enhancers may destroy what is perceived as normal or considered natural in humans. The extent to which science and scientific technologies can intervene with human life, life expectancy, and morbidity have always been accompanied by ongoing discussion regarding ethics and morality [79,80]. One of the difficulties with the debate of the application of TMS in healthy individuals is the lack of consensus around the extent to which an enhancement may be considered acceptable or natural. Scientists who contribute to the development of newer technologies such as TMS need to consider the ongoing moral questions, such as whether cognitive enhancements imply a sense of hierarchy or deportation from what is considered human nature.

Second, the use of cognitive enhancers that are not yet regulated may lead to discrimination among humans, as disparity in fairness between individuals can arise as a result of the accessibility to TMS. Moreover, if the cognitive enhancing effects of TMS is validated as permanent in the near future, discrimination including negative social backlash may arise, which will warrant for stricter regulations and standards within fields related to cognitive performance, such as test-taking environments [81]. In contrast, there may also be societal pressure to attain cognitive enhancement via neurostimulatory devices, whether this enforcement is a collective familial belief within a household, a provider's belief from an employer's perspective, or cultural belief. The difficult line between consensual and enforcement will then need to be considered, where the enhancement of one's cognitive function may soon be associated with desirability in one's personal or professional life. Considering this, whether one aims to have their cognitive function improved via TMS may not be a personal choice, but rather an obligation for the purpose of success or meeting the demands of others.

Finally, under the assumption that cognitive enhancement is possible, the matter of fairness may take a toll in the competitive society in which we live. If TMS can in fact provide irreversible cognitive enhancement to neurally intact individuals, the possibility of privatizing TMS as a neurostimulatory tool for societal gains arises, where TMS may be commercialized for economic gains. As demands for TMS increase, there may be a socioeconomic class gap between those who have access to TMS and those who do not, eventually leading to unjust labor standards. In addition, individuals with favorable traits in relation to cognition, such as higher attentiveness or processing skills, may be considered moot as anyone can potentially afford to attain such traits. Furthermore, the stratification between those who can and cannot use this scientific tool may be deepened, where this stratification may not be limited to between socioeconomic classes, but also across cultures and countries, resulting in a global disparity in distribution.

The current manuscript reviews with perspective the cognitive enhancement effects of TMS that has been demonstrated in recent research. The studies discussed in this review not only confirm the effects of TMS treatment in cognitive improvement, but also suggest that the effects of TMS may be specific to the method of stimulation. Overall, the studies have shown that the enhancing effects of rTMS are not task-specific or specific to the target group, but rather frequency and area-specific, as suggested in previous studies also [48]. Therefore, the use of TMS as treatment may be a valid direction in future research, as it may have universal enhancement effects across individuals regardless of clinical factors such as illnesses including but not limited to depression, schizophrenia, and cognitive impairment.

However, it is important to note that there have been several studies that dispute the cognitive enhancing effects of rTMS as mentioned above (Table 3). Over the years, a number of studies conducted in various target groups including healthy individuals [82], those with depression [51,83,84,85], and schizophrenia [86], have demonstrated no significant change in cognitive performance. While most of these studies have used LF-rTMS unlike the studies that demonstrated significant cognitive enhancement via HF-rTMS, the fact that TMS may have no clinical effect in cognition under certain protocols should be noted.

In addition, the studies currently reviewed mostly investigated the short-term and immediate effects of TMS in the enhancement of cognition. While a few studies have undergone a 2-week and 1-month follow-up assessment, respectively, they have suggested that the effects of TMS may be short-term in cognitive domains regarding executive functioning while delayed or sustained in processing skills [48,50]. To clarify these findings, the development and establishment of a universal protocol in the application of HF-rTMS for the purpose of cognitive enhancement is warranted, as some studies have suggested that a minimum of 4-week treatment may be necessary to see significant effects of TMS [39,87]. Future studies that are more controlled in the application processes of rTMS are needed to specify the effects of rTMS with regards to cognitive impairment, including the optimal frequency, amount of pulses delivered, intertrain interval, and area of application. Furthermore, many of the TMS studies on cognitive enhancement lack a healthy comparison group as part of the study design. In order to ensure that the cognitive enhancing effects of those with psychiatric or neurological conditions are due to TMS, rather than simply an improvement of psychiatric symptoms, ongoing controlled studies with age- and sex-matched healthy controls are warranted.

It is also noteworthy that having a consistent method of assessment for cognitive function may support in increasing the reliability of the study findings, such as using a test battery that has been well supported over the years including the CANTAB. Through the review of recent TMS studies, it may demonstrate that there are very few number of studies that investigate the cognitive function of individuals as a primary outcome variable. These studies have primarily focused on utilizing neuropsychological tests as means to observe for any deterring of cognitive abilities, and therefore the tests may be less sensitive to detecting improvement.

In conclusion, TMS is a phenomenal technology that has endless potentials to human cognition and enhancement that is alternative to pharmaceutical research. To investigate the effects of TMS beyond the well-established domain of motor function improvement and more towards the cognitive enhancing effects, further clinical trials and optimized protocols are warranted. Future research including randomized controlled trials and clinical trials using various parameters of rTMS may contribute in the development of an optimized protocol for rTMS in the treatment of and enhancement of cognitive abilities, an innovative approach which may open up new directions in the perceptualization of human cognition.

Fig. 1. Schematic diagram of the underlying mechanism of rTMS. (A) Three-dimensional images of the brain that highlight the frontoparietal network of the brain. The top image shows the axial view of the brain, alongside with the area of stimulation for the rTMS treatment summarized in the current review colored in light green. The bottom image shows the sagittal view of the brain. Each circle is a node that represents the region of the brain that is part of the frontoparietal network, and each grey line is an edge that represents the inter-regional connectivity between the nodes. (B) The two potential neural pathways that underlie rTMS treatment to the DLPFC. A T1-weighted image of the brain is shown in sagittal, coronal, and axial view to show the DLPFC as highlighted in yellow. R, right; L, left; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; IPS, inferior parietal sulcus; CC, corpus callosum; rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; FPN, frontoparietal network; DMN, default mode network.
Table. 1. Studies that have shown increase in memory after rTMS treatment
GroupReferenceNLocationSession/WeekFrequency (Hz)Train Duration (s)Trains per SessionIntertraininterval (s)Pulses per sessionMT (%)Cognitive Rating
HealthyGagnon et al. (2011)11R, L DLPFC1/--Paired-pulse-0.015-90Remember/Know Task
HealthyBagherzadeh et al. (2016)30L DLPFC10/2101-5600100Digit span, PRM, DMS
DepTriggs et al. (1999)10L PFC10/22025028200080Digit span, COWA
DepFitzgerald et al. (2003)60L DLPFC20/416425300100Digit span, Verbal fluency
105201000 Semantic & Autobiographical
DepBoggio et al. (2005)25L DLPFC10/21554025-110WCST
DepFabre et al. (2004)11L PFC10/21082052-100Hive Test
DepHansen et al. (2011)30R PFC15/31602180-110Digit Symbol, Rey
DepWajdik et al. (2014)63L DLPFC15/310532251600110RAVLT
PTSDBoggio et al. (2010)30R, L DLPFC10/22024028160080WCST, COWA, DSP
SchSchneider et al. (2008)51L DLPFC20/410---1000110WCST
SchBarr et al. (2011)*46L DLPFC1/-204252575090N-back test
MCISole-Padulles et al. (2006)39L PMC1/-510102-8010-block design task
MCIDrumond Marra et al. (2015)34L DLPFC10/2105-252000110RBMT
AphaThiel et al. (2013)24Posterior IFG10/2112001--90Token test, naming, writing
ADZhao et al. (2017)30Posterior T30/620102020--ADAS-Cog

Dep, depression; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; Sch, schizophrenia; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; Apha, aphasia; AD, Alzheimer's Disease; L, left; R, right; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PMC, primary motor cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; T, temporal; PRM, pattern recognition memory; DMS, delayed matching to sample; WCST, Wisconsin card sorting test; RAVLT, rey auditory verbal learning test; COWA, controlled oral word association; RBMT, rivermead behavioural memory test; ADAS-Cog, Alzheimer's disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale.

*Indicates the inclusion of a healthy comparison group.


Table. 2. Studies that have shown increase in processing skills after rTMS treatment
GroupReferenceNLocationSession per WeekFrequency (Hz)Train Duration (s)Trains per SessionIntertraininterval (s)Pulses per sessionMT (%)Cognitive Rating
HealthyVanderhasselt et al. (2006)28L DLPFC1/-1044026.11560110Stroop task
HealthyVanderhasselt et al. (2007)20R DLPFC1/-1044026.11560110Stroop task
HealthyHwang et al. (2010)17L DLPFC1/-102-1090090Conners' CPT
DepGrunhaus et al. (2000)20L Motor Cortex20/4106--120090TMT-B
DepMoser et al. (2002)19L MFG5/12022060-80TMT-B
DepMartis et al. (2003)15L PFC15/310520301000110Stroop task, SCRT, GP
DepHoppner et al. (2003)30R DLPFC10/21602180-110MARS
L DLPFC2022060
DepHausmann et al. (2004)41R, L DLPFC10/216001-2600120Stroop task
201010902000100
DepBoggio et al. (2005)25L DLPFC10/21554025-110Stroop task
DepKedzior et al. (2012)-L DLPFC40/310540252000100mCST
DepMyczkowski et al. (2012)14L DLPFC20/451025201250120TMT-B, Stroop task
PTSDBoggio et al. (2010)30R, L DLPFC10/22024028160080Stroop task
SchLevkovitz et al. (2011)15R L PFC20/420----120SOC, SSP, SWM
SchGuse et al. (2013)*47L DLPFC15/310-25301000110N-back test, DAT
DemAntczak et al. (2018)11R L DLPFC10/21052025300090LCT, Stroop task

Dep, depression; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; Sch, schizophrenia; Dem, dementia; L, left; R, right; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; PFC, prefrontal cortex; CPT, continuous performance task; TMT, trail making test; SCRT, simple and choice reaction time; GP, grooved pegboard; MARS, motor agitation and retardation scale; mCST, modified concept shifting task; SOC, stockings of Cambridge; SSP, spatial span; SWM, spatial working memory; DAT, divided attention task; LCT, Letter Cancellation Test.

*Indicates the inclusion of a healthy comparison group.


Table. 3. Studies that have not shown significant differences in cognitive function after rTMS treatment
GroupReferenceNLocationSession per WeekFrequency (Hz)Train Duration (s)Trains per SessionIntertraininterval (s)Pulses per sessionMT (%)Cognitive Domain
DepSpeer et al. (2001)18L DLPFC10/220240281600100Memory (Episodic, Visuospatial, Semantic)
DepLoo et al. (2001)18L DLPFC10/21053025-110Working memory
Verbal fluency
Dep+PTSDRosenberg et al. (2002)12L DLPFC10/21404020600090Episodic Memory
5852USC-REMT
DepMartis et al. (2003)15L PFC15/31052030-110Attentional control
 Verbal fluency
 Working memory
DepJanuel et al. (2006)27R DLPFC16/41602180-90Attentional control
Executive functioning
DepFitzgerald et al. (2009)27L PFC15/31180430-110Memory (Visuospatial, Working)
1053025Verbal fluency
SchHoffman et al. (2005)50L TPC18/219601-200090Attentional control
 Memory (Verbal, Working)
 Verbal fluency
SchFitzgerald et al. (2005)33L TPC10/219001--90Memory (Visuospatial, Working)
Verbal fluency
OCDKang et al. (2009)20R DLPFC10/2112001--110Verbal fluency Attentional Control
StrokeKim et al. (2010)18-10/2130046090080Executive functioning
1011510450
AphWaldowski et al. (2012)26R IFG15/3118001--90Verbal fluency

Dep, depression; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; Sch, schizophrenia; OCD, obsessive compulsive disorder; Aph, aphasia; L, left; R, right; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; TPC, temporoparietal cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; USC-REMT, University of Southern California repeatable episodic memory test.


  1. Stahnisch FW. In: Clausen J, Levy N. Handbook of neuroethics. Springer: Dordrecht, 2015; 2015. p. 461-466.
  2. Flanagan SR, Cantor JB, Ashman TA. Traumatic brain injury: future assessment tools and treatment prospects. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2008;4:877-892.
    Pubmed
  3. Emerson RW, Adams C, Nishino T, Hazlett HC, Wolff JJ, Zwaigenbaum L, Constantino JN, Shen MD, Swanson MR, Elison JT, Kandala S, Estes AM, Botteron KN, Collins L, Dager SR, Evans AC, Gerig G, Gu H, McKinstry RC, Paterson S, Schultz RT, Styner M;, Schlagger BL, Pruett JR, Piven J. Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Sci Transl Med 2017;9:eaag2882.
    Pubmed
  4. Shahbasak PP, Hamilton RH. In: Ter Meulen R, Mohammed A, Hall W. Rethinking cognitive enhancement. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2017; 2017. p. 125-149.
  5. Ribrary U, MacKay AL, Rauscher A, Tipper CM, Giaschi DE, Woodward TS, Sossi V, Doesburg SM, Ward LM, Herdman A, Hamarneh G, Booth BG, Moiseev A. In: Illes J. Neuroethics: anticipating the future. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2017; 2017. p. 15-53.
  6. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1336-1346.
    Pubmed
  7. Hausmann A, Pascual-Leone A, Kemmler G, Rupp CI, Lechner-Schoner T, Kramer-Reinstadler K, Walpoth M, Mechtcheriakov S, Conca A, Weiss EM. No deterioration of cognitive performance in an aggressive unilateral and bilateral antidepressant rTMS add-on trial. J Clin Psychiatry 2004;65:772-782.
    Pubmed
  8. Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2014;44:2029-2040.
    Pubmed
  9. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;44:377-384.
    Pubmed
  10. Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, Kuck J, Marcotte TD, Jeste DV. Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:24-32.
    Pubmed
  11. Hoffman RE, Wu K, Pittman B, Cahill JD, Hawkins KA, Fernandez T, Hannestad J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of Wernicke's and right homologous sites to curtail “voices”: a randomized trial. Biol Psychiatry 2013;73:1008-1014.
    Pubmed
  12. McAllister TW, Zafonte R, Jain S, Flashman LA, George MS, Grant GA, He F, Lohr JB, Andaluz N, Summerall L, Paulus MP, Raman R, Stein MB. Randomized placebo-controlled trial of methylphenidate or galantamine for persistent emotional and cognitive symptoms associated with PTSD and/or traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016;41:1191-1198.
    Pubmed
  13. Chard KM, Schumm JA, McIlvain SM, Bailey GW, Parkinson RB. Exploring the efficacy of a residential treatment program incorporating cognitive processing therapy-cognitive for veterans with PTSD and traumatic brain injury. J Trauma Stress 2011;24:347-351.
    Pubmed
  14. Su H, Zhong N, Gan H, Wang J, Han H, Chen T, Li X, Ruan X, Zhu Y, Jiang H, Zhao M. High frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for methamphetamine use disorders: A randomised clinical trial. Drug Alcohol Depend 2017;175:84-91.
    Pubmed
  15. Sahakian BJ, Morein-Zamir S. Neuroethical issues in cognitive enhancement. J Psychopharmacol 2011;25:197-204.
    Pubmed
  16. Dresler M, Sandberg A, Ohla K, Bublitz C, Trenado C, Mroczko-Wąsowicz A, Kühn S, Repantis D. Nonpharmacological cognitive enhancement. Neuropharmacology 2013;64:529-543.
    Pubmed
  17. Appel JM. When the boss turns pusher: a proposal for employee protections in the age of cosmetic neurology. J Med Ethics 2008;34:616-618.
    Pubmed
  18. Cakic V. Smart drugs for cognitive enhancement: ethical and pragmatic considerations in the era of cosmetic neurology. J Med Ethics 2009;35:611-615.
    Pubmed
  19. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich R, Carter M, Greenwald D, Pogue-Geile M, Kechavan M, Cooley S, DiBarry AL, Garrett A, Parepally H, Zoretich R. Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:866-876.
    Pubmed
  20. Duffy R, Wiseman H, File SE. Improved cognitive function in postmenopausal women after 12 weeks of consumption of a soya extract containing isoflavones. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;75:721-729.
    Pubmed
  21. Snowball A, Tachtsidis I, Popescu T, Thompson J, Delazer M, Zamarian L, Zhu T, Cohen Kadosh R. Long-term enhancement of brain function and cognition using cognitive training and brain stimulation. Curr Biol 2013;23:987-992.
    Pubmed
  22. Fregni F, Pascual-Leone A. Technology insight: noninvasive brain stimulation in neurology-perspectives on the therapeutic potential of rTMS and tDCS. Nat Clin Pract Neurol 2007;3:383-393.
    Pubmed
  23. George MS, Aston-Jones G. Noninvasive techniques for probing neurocircuitry and treating illness: vagus nerve stimulation (VNS), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35:301-316.
    Pubmed
  24. Henrich-Noack P, Sergeeva EG, Sabel BA. Noninvasive electrical brain stimulation: from acute to late-stage treatment of central nervous system damage. Neural Regen Res 2017;12:1590-1594.
    Pubmed
  25. Kuffler DP. Coping with phantom limb pain. Mol Neurobiol 2018;55:70-84.
    Pubmed
  26. O'Reardon JP, Solvason HB, Janicak PG, Sampson S, Isenberg KE, Nahas Z, McDonald WM, Avery D, Fitzgerald PB, Loo C, Demitrack MA, George MS, Sackeim HA. Efficacy and safety of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the acute treatment of major depression: a multisite randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:1208-1216.
    Pubmed
  27. Gagnon G, Schneider C, Grondin S, Blanchet S. Enhancement of episodic memory in young and healthy adults: a paired-pulse TMS study on encoding and retrieval performance. Neurosci Lett 2011;488:138-142.
    Pubmed
  28. Yamanaka K, Yamagata B, Tomioka H, Kawasaki S, Mimura M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex facilitates spatial working memory: near-infrared spectroscopy study. Cereb Cortex 2010;20:1037-1045.
    Pubmed
  29. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Gerloff C. Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. Eur J Neurosci 2003;17:1129-1133.
    Pubmed
  30. Boyd LA, Linsdell MA. Excitatory repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to left dorsal premotor cortex enhances motor consolidation of new skills. BMC Neurosci 2009;10:72.
    Pubmed
  31. Hwang JH, Kim SH, Park CS, Bang SA, Kim SE. Acute high-frequency rTMS of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and attentional control in healthy young men. Brain Res 2010;1329:152-158.
    Pubmed
  32. Luber B, Lisanby SH. Enhancement of human cognitive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neuroimage 2014;85:961-970.
    Pubmed
  33. Aydın EP, Genç A, Dalkıran M, Uyar ET, Deniz İ, Özer OA, Karamustafalıoğlu KO. Thioredoxin is not a marker for treatment-resistance depression but associated with cognitive function: An rTMS study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2018;80:322-328.
    Pubmed
  34. Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature 1999;399:148-151.
    Pubmed
  35. Terao Y, Ugawa Y. Basic mechanisms of TMS. J Clin Neurophysiol 2002;19:322-343.
    Pubmed
  36. Rossini PM, Rossi S. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: diagnostic, therapeutic, and research potential. Neurology 2007;68:484-488.
    Pubmed
  37. Ridding MC, Brouwer B, Miles TS, Pitcher JB, Thompson PD. Changes in muscle responses to stimulation of the motor cortex induced by peripheral nerve stimulation in human subjects. Exp Brain Res 2000;131:135-143.
    Pubmed
  38. Guse B, Falkai P, Gruber O, Whalley H, Gibson L, Hasan A, Obst K, Dechent P, McIntosh A, Suchan B, Wobrock T. The effect of long-term high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on working memory in schizophrenia and healthy controls--a randomized placebo-controlled, double-blind fMRI study. Behav Brain Res 2013;237:300-307.
    Pubmed
  39. Waldowski K, Seniów J, Leśniak M, Iwański S, Członkowska A. Effect of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on naming abilities in early-stroke aphasic patients: a prospective, randomized, double-blind shamcontrolled study. ScientificWorldJournal 2012;2012:518568.
    Pubmed
  40. Song S, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Modifying somatosensory processing with non-invasive brain stimulation. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2011;29:427-437.
    Pubmed
  41. Elahi B, Elahi B, Chen R. Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on Parkinson motor function--systematic review of controlled clinical trials. Mov Disord 2009;24:357-363.
    Pubmed
  42. Slotema CW, Blom JD, Hoek HW, Sommer IE. Should we expand the toolbox of psychiatric treatment methods to include repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS)? A meta-analysis of the efficacy of rTMS in psychiatric disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 2010;71:873-884.
    Pubmed
  43. Clark C, Cole J, Winter C, Williams K, Grammer G. A review of transcranial magnetic stimulation as a treatment for post-traumatic stress disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2015;17:83.
    Pubmed
  44. Nadeau SE, Bowers D, Jones TL, Wu SS, Triggs WJ, Heilman KM. Cognitive effects of treatment of depression with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cogn Behav Neurol 2014;27:77-87.
    Pubmed
  45. Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R, Baeken C, Leyman L, D'haenen H. The influence of rTMS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on Stroop task performance. Exp Brain Res 2006;169:279-282.
    Pubmed
  46. Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R, Baeken C, Leyman L, Clerinx P, D'haenen H. The influence of rTMS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on top-down attentional processes. Brain Res 2007;1137:111-116.
    Pubmed
  47. Antczak J, Kowalska K, Klimkowicz-Mrowiec A, Wach B, Kasprzyk K, Banach M, Rzeźnicka-Brzegowy K, Kubica J, Słowik A. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of cognitive impairment in frontotemporal dementia: an open-label pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018;14:749-755.
    Pubmed
  48. Levkovitz Y, Rabany L, Harel EV, Zangen A. Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation add-on for treatment of negative symptoms and cognitive deficits of schizophrenia: a feasibility study. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011;14:991-996.
    Pubmed
  49. Bagherzadeh Y, Khorrami A, Zarrindast MR, Shariat SV, Pantazis D. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp Brain Res 2016;234:1807-1818.
    Pubmed
  50. Drumond Marra HL, Myczkowski ML, Maia Memória C, Arnaut D, Leite Ribeiro P, Sardinha Mansur CG, Lancelote Alberto R, Boura Bellini B, Alves Fernandes da Silva A, Tortella G, Ciampi de Andrade D, Teixeira MJ, Forlenza OV, Marcolin MA. Transcranial magnetic stimulation to address mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: a randomized controlled study. Behav Neurol 2015;2015:287843.
    Pubmed
  51. Fitzgerald PB, Brown TL, Marston NA, Daskalakis ZJ, De Castella A, Kulkarni J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression: a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:1002-1008.
    Pubmed
  52. Farzan F, Barr MS, Sun Y, Fitzgerald PB, Daskalakis ZJ. Transcranial magnetic stimulation on the modulation of gamma oscillations in schizophrenia. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012;1265:25-35.
    Pubmed
  53. Solé-Padullés C, Bartrés-Faz D, Junqué C, Clemente IC, Molinuevo JL, Bargalló N, Sánchez-Aldeguer J, Bosch B, Falcón C, Valls-Solé J. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on brain function and cognition among elders with memory dysfunction. A randomized sham-controlled study. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:1487-1493.
    Pubmed
  54. Meeker MN. The structure of intellect, its interpretations and usesArray. Columbus, OH: Merrill Publishing Company, 1969.
  55. Baddeley A. Working memory. Science 1992;255:556-559.
    Pubmed
  56. Forbes NF, Carrick LA, McIntosh AM, Lawrie SM. Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2009;39:889-905.
    Pubmed
  57. Barr MS, Farzan F, Arenovich T, Chen R, Fitzgerald PB, Daskalakis ZJ. The effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia. PLoS One 2011;6:e22627.
    Pubmed
  58. Hansen PE, Ravnkilde B, Videbech P, Clemmensen K, Sturlason R, Reiner M, Parner E, Rosenberg R, Vestergaard P. Low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation inferior to electroconvulsive therapy in treating depression. J ECT 2011;27:26-32.
    Pubmed
  59. Munera CP, Lomlomdjian C, Gori B, Terpiluk V, Medel N, Solis P, Kochen S. Episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res Treat 2014;2014:157452.
    Pubmed
  60. Murphy KJ, Troyer AK, Levine B, Moscovitch M. Episodic, but not semantic, autobiographical memory is reduced in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:3116-3123.
    Pubmed
  61. Melnick MD, Harrison BR, Park S, Bennetto L, Tadin D. A strong interactive link between sensory discriminations and intelligence. Curr Biol 2013;23:1013-1017.
    Pubmed
  62. Höppner J, Schulz M, Irmisch G, Mau R, Schläfke D, Richter J. Antidepressant efficacy of two different rTMS procedures. High frequency over left versus low frequency over right prefrontal cortex compared with sham stimulation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003;253:103-109.
    Pubmed
  63. Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel SA. Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat Neurosci 2000;3:1149-1152.
    Pubmed
  64. Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Gong QY, Roberts N, Kapur N. Item recognition is less impaired than recall and associative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal damage. Hippocampus 2005;15:203-215.
    Pubmed
  65. Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol 2013;23:R764-R773.
    Pubmed
  66. Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, Dörfel D, Rohleder N, Werner A. A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:1004-1031.
    Pubmed
  67. Boggio PS, Fregni F, Bermpohl F, Mansur CG, Rosa M, Rumi DO, Barbosa ER, Odebrecht Rosa M, Pascual-Leone A, Rigonatti SP, Marcolin MA, Araujo Silva MT. Effect of repetitive TMS and fluoxetine on cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease and concurrent depression. Mov Disord 2005;20:1178-1184.
    Pubmed
  68. Fabre I, Galinowski A, Oppenheim C, Gallarda T, Meder JF, De Montigny C, Olié JP, Poirier MF. Antidepressant efficacy and cognitive effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in vascular depression: an open trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2004;19:833-842.
    Pubmed
  69. Wajdik C, Claypoole KH, Fawaz W, Holtzheimer PE, Neumaier J, Dunner DL, Haynor DR, Roy-Byrne P, Avery DH. No change in neuropsychological functioning after receiving repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for major depression. J ECT 2014;30:320-324.
    Pubmed
  70. Boggio PS, Rocha M, Oliveira MO, Fecteau S, Cohen RB, Campanhã C, Ferreira-Santos E, Meleiro A, Corchs F, Zaghi S, Pascual-Leone A, Fregni F. Noninvasive brain stimulation with high-frequency and low-intensity repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2010;71:992-999.
    Pubmed
  71. Grunhaus L, Dannon PN, Schreiber S, Dolberg OH, Amiaz R, Ziv R, Lefkifker E. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation is as effective as electroconvulsive therapy in the treatment of nondelusional major depressive disorder: an open study. Biol Psychiatry 2000;47:314-324.
    Pubmed
  72. Moser DJ, Jorge RE, Manes F, Paradiso S, Benjamin ML, Robinson RG. Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 2002;58:1288-1290.
    Pubmed
  73. Myczkowski ML, Dias AM, Luvisotto T, Arnaut D, Bellini BB, Mansur CG, Rennó J, Tortella G, Ribeiro PL, Marcolin MA. Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on clinical, social, and cognitive performance in postpartum depression. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2012;8:491-500.
    Pubmed
  74. Martis B, Alam D, Dowd SM, Hill SK, Sharma RP, Rosen C, Pliskin N, Martin E, Carson V, Janicak PG. Neurocognitive effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in severe major depression. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:1125-1132.
    Pubmed
  75. Bridge DJ, Cohen NJ, Voss JL. Distinct hippocampal versus frontoparietal network contributions to retrieval and memory-guided exploration. J Cogn Neurosci 2017;29:1324-1338.
    Pubmed
  76. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008;1124:1-38.
    Pubmed
  77. Utevsky AV, Smith DV, Huettel SA. Precuneus is a functional core of the default-mode network. J Neurosci 2014;34:932-940.
    Pubmed
  78. Peterchev AV, Wagner TA, Miranda PC, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Lisanby SH, Pascual-Leone A, Bikson M. Fundamentals of transcranial electric and magnetic stimulation dose: definition, selection, and reporting practices. Brain Stimul 2012;5:435-453.
    Pubmed
  79. Blank RH. Cognitive enhancement: social and public policy issues. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 2015.
  80. Blank RH. In: Blank RH. Cognitive enhancement: social and public policy issues. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 2016; 2016. p. 1-41.
  81. Miah A. In: Gonzalez F. Values and ethics for the 21st century. Bilbao: BBVA, 2011; 2011. p. 199-231.
  82. Daskalakis ZJ, Möller B, Christensen BK, Fitzgerald PB, Gunraj C, Chen R. The effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical inhibition in healthy human subjects. Exp Brain Res 2006;174:403-412.
    Pubmed
  83. Speer AM, Repella JD, Figueras S, Demian NK, Kimbrell TA, Wasserman EM, Post RM. Lack of adverse cognitive effects of 1 Hz and 20 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at 100% of motor threshold over left prefrontal cortex in depression. J ECT 2001;17:259-263.
    Pubmed
  84. Januel D, Dumortier G, Verdon CM, Stamatiadis L, Saba G, Cabaret W, Benadhira R, Rocamora JF, Braha S, Kalalou K, Vicaut PE, Fermanian J. A double-blind sham controlled study of right prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): therapeutic and cognitive effect in medication free unipolar depression during 4 weeks. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2006;30:126-130.
    Pubmed
  85. Fitzgerald PB, Hoy K, Daskalakis ZJ, Kulkarni J. A randomized trial of the anti-depressant effects of low- and high-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation in treatment-resistant depression. Depress Anxiety 2009;26:229-234.
    Pubmed
  86. Hoffman RE, Gueorguieva R, Hawkins KA, Varanko M, Boutros NN, Wu YT, Carroll K, Krystal JH. Temporoparietal transcranial magnetic stimulation for auditory hallucinations: safety, efficacy and moderators in a fifty patient sample. Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:97-104.
    Pubmed
  87. Triggs WJ, McCoy KJ, Greer R, Rossi F, Bowers D, Kortenkamp S, Nadeau SE, Heilman KM, Goodman WK. Effects of left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation on depressed mood, cognition, and corticomotor threshold. Biol Psychiatry 1999;45:1440-1446.
    Pubmed
  88. Schneider AL, Schneider TL, Stark H. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) as an augmentation treatment for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a 4-week randomized placebo controlled study. Brain Stimul 2008;1:106-111.
    Pubmed
  89. Thiel A, Hartmann A, Rubi-Fessen I, Anglade C, Kracht L, Weiduschat N, Kessler J, Rommel T, Heiss WD. Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on language networks and recovery in early poststroke aphasia. Stroke 2013;44:2240-2246.
    Pubmed
  90. Zhao J, Li Z, Cong Y, Zhang J, Tan M, Zhang H, Geng N, Li M, Yu W, Shan P. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation improves cognitive function of Alzheimer's disease patients. Oncotarget 2017;8:33864-33871.
    Pubmed
  91. Kedzior KK, Rajput V, Price G, Lee J, Martin-Iverson M. Cognitive correlates of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in treatment-resistant depression--a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry 2012;12:163.
    Pubmed
  92. Loo C, Sachdev P, Elsayed H, McDarmont B, Mitchell P, Wilkinson M, Parker G, Gandevia S. Effects of a 2- to 4-week course of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on neuropsychologic functioning, electroencephalogram, and auditory threshold in depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry 2001;49:615-623.
    Pubmed
  93. Rosenberg PB, Mehndiratta RB, Mehndiratta YP, Wamer A, Rosse RB, Balish M. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment of comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and major depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;14:270-276.
    Pubmed
  94. Fitzgerald PB, Benitez J, Daskalakis JZ, Brown TL, Marston NA, de Castella A, Kulkarni J. A double-blind shamcontrolled trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of refractory auditory hallucinations. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2005;25:358-362.
    Pubmed
  95. Kang JI, Kim CH, Namkoong K, Lee CI, Kim SJ. A randomized controlled study of sequentially applied repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2009;70:1645-1651.
    Pubmed
  96. Kim BR, Kim DY, Chun MH, Yi JH, Kwon JS. Effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognition and mood in stroke patients: a double-blind, sham-controlled trial. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2010;89:362-368.
    Pubmed