View Full Text | Abstract |
Article as PDF | Print this Article |
Pubmed | PMC |
PubReader | Export to Citation |
Email Alerts | Open Access |
Exp Neurobiol 2021; 30(1): 48-58
Published online February 28, 2021
https://doi.org/10.5607/en20038
© The Korean Society for Brain and Neural Sciences
Taesub Jung, Minji Jang and Jihyun Noh*
Department of Science Education, Dankook University, Yongin 16890, Korea
Correspondence to: *To whom correspondence should be addressed.
TEL: 82-31-8005-3842, FAX: 82-31-8021-7231
e-mail: jihyun2@dankook.ac.kr
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Fear-related behaviors are rigidly controlled by the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). The mPFC is activated by the prosocial hormone oxytocin, which plays an important role in social buffering. We used a slice patch current-clamp recording in single- and pair-exposed rats who were subjected to electric shocks, to determine the cellular mechanism of the action of oxytocin in the mPFC under social buffering conditions. Pair-exposed rats showed a significant reduction in both freezing and passive avoidance behaviors compared to single-exposed rats. It was observed that input resistance in pyramidal neurons decreased in both single- and pair-exposed rats than na?ve rats, but input resistance in interneurons increased in pair-exposed rats than single-exposed rats. We found that the number of action potential (AP) spikes in the mPFC pyramidal neurons decreased significantly in pair-exposed rats than in single-exposed rats. The pyramidal neurons in the mPFC were similarly regulated by oxytocin in singleand pair-exposed rats, while the number of AP spikes in interneurons by oxytocin decreased in single-exposed rats, but there was no significant change in pair-exposed rats. Therefore, our findings reveal that a decrease in mPFC pyramidal neuronal activity in pair-exposed rats through social interaction induces a reduction in fear-related behavior via obstruction of fear-memory formation; however, no such reduction was observed in single-exposed rats. Moreover, we suggest that the oxytocin-mediated decrease in neuronal activity in the mPFC could facilitate social buffering.
Keywords: Cortical neuron, Fear response, Oxytocin, Prefrontal cortex, Social buffering
Social buffering plays an important role as a positive factor in the formation of social relationships and bonding [1]. Social buffering can be defined as the phenomenon of improved recovery from stressful experiences when an organism is in the company of members of the conspecific species; this phenomenon is observed in rats, mice, birds, apes, and humans. Social buffering is similarly observed in humans and improves human health as part of the benefits of social support [2]. Social buffering affects the stabilization of the emotional state, which can function to alleviate negative emotional behaviors such as anxiety and depression [3]. The positive effects of this social buffering are the focus of several ongoing studies, but the mechanism is mainly thought to be a hormone-driven one. Moreover, the mechanisms and specific brain areas associated with this phenomenon are still insufficiently understood.
The stress-induced release of corticosterone is reduced by social buffering, while the positive effects of the social buffering process can affect conditioning or extinction of fear memory, which is classified as a stress memory [4-6]. These social buffering-induced stress-relieving effects are mediated by various areas of the brain, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), amygdala, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, paraventricular nucleus, and nucleus accumbens [7]. The roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in empathy through the mirror neuron system, the prelimbic region (PL) in social recognition, and the infralimbic (IL) cortex in social memory suggest that the mPFC is related to social buffering associated with oxytocin [8-10]. The oxytocin receptor-expressed neurons are distributed in the mPFC, and social buffering can alter the distribution, number, and function of oxytocin neurons [11, 12]. Moreover, social buffering induces an increase in the oxytocin receptors in oxytocin neurons in the amygdala [13]. The role of oxytocin as a social hormone and that of dopamine with reward-seeking behaviors are important for social bonding and social buffering. The mPFC plays an important role in controlling oxytocin and dopamine neurocircuitry [14, 15]. Therefore, this study focused on the role of mPFC neurons under social buffering conditions and their change of oxytocin.
Microinfusion of an oxytocin agonist into the mPFC enhanced fear extinction, whereas the oxytocin antagonist impaired fear extinction and reversed the enhancing effect of pairing on fear extinction [16]. One study showed that oxytocin played an important role in social recognition memory: depletion of oxytocin in the mPFC of rats hampered their ability to distinguish between novel mice and familiar mice [17]. Excitation of the oxytocin-sensitive PFC to the connection with the basolateral amygdala (BLA) using in vivo optogenetic techniques eliminated the preference of social novelty, suggesting that this circuit is required for social recognition [18]. Therefore, it is essential to verify the modulation of the mPFC after exposure to oxytocin to understand the functions of the latter.
Previously, pair-exposed rats who were kept in the company of conspecific organisms for social buffering were subjected to fear conditioning and underwent a passive avoidance test to evaluate memory function and freezing behavior, to demonstrate whether social buffering induces the link between fear responses and fear memory from social stimuli during fear conditioning [19]. Previous studies have identified that pair exposure to conspecific organisms during fear conditioning helps the organism to cope with both the freezing-response and the fear memory systems; however, no study has focused on their underlying mechanism. In this study, a pair-exposed fear model was used to investigate the role of social buffering on the fear-related response and a whole-cell patch recording was conducted to verify the brain function mechanism of social buffering, with a special focus on the oxytocin modulation of the function of both pyramidal neurons and interneurons.
Subjects were Sprague–Dawley adolescent male rats (Postnatal Days 21~42) from Orient Bio (Seongnam, Republic of Korea). All experiments were conducted by the guidelines of the ethics committee of the Dankook University Ethics Committee’s Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (DKU-17-033). Rats were weaned at 21 days of age and individually housed in a Plexiglas cage (46×23×20 cm) with wood bedding with a 12:12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 9 A.M.). For paired shock exposure, separated weaned siblings were used. Food and water were always available. The experiments were conducted in a sound-insulated compartment with controlled temperature (23±1°C) and humidity (45±5%) following the experimental schematic timeline shown in Fig. 1A. The rats were assigned to one of the following two treated groups: (1) Single-exposed rats, which were shocked alone in a learning session; (2) Pair-exposed rats, which were shocked with their mate in a learning session; (3) Naïve rats, which were not exposed to electric shock.
The passive avoidance test consists of habituation, learning, and retention session (Fig. 1A). All rats habituated the test environment 10 min. In the habituation session, rats were placed in a white compartment and could explore both black and white compartments to estimate which compartment they preferred for 5 min. After 1 day, rats were placed in a white compartment for 1 min and the door which blocked gate of black and white compartment was opened. When rats passed from the white compartment to the black compartment, the door was closed and then rats could not go back the white compartment again and received electrical shock delivered through the grids (0.7 mA for 5 s; learning session) [19]. After 1 day, rats were placed in the white compartment for 1 min and the door was opened. During the retention session, the duration of freezing, black compartment preference, number of the entry, and latency into the black compartment for 5 min were measured. The passive avoidance box was cleaned with 70% ethanol after each trial.
After passive avoidance test, brains were quickly removed and immersed in an oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2), ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) solution containing the following composition (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1.3 MgSO4, 2.4 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, and 8 D-glucose. Coronal mPFC slices (300-µm) were cut using a vibratome (5,100 mz-22, Campden Instrument, England). During 15 min, brain slices were incubated with warm (30°C) oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) N-Methyl-D-glucamine (NMDG) ACSF containing the following (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 10 MgSO4, 0.5 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 25 D-glucose, 110 NMDG, and 80.05 HCl. Then, slices were incubated with oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) ACSF for >1 h at room temperature. Recordings were performed in a submersion-type chamber (2~4 ml/min perfusion of oxygenated ACSF at 33~35°C) mounted on an upright microscope (Nikon Eclipse FN1, Tokyo, Japan). Recorded action potential (AP) spikes in the mPFC region using a borosilicate glass capillary electrode with internal solution connected to a Multi-Clamp Amplifier (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA, USA). Recording pipettes were filled with intracellular solution (in mM): 120 K-MeSO3, 10 Hepes, 0.25 EGTA, 2 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 10 Na2-Phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, and 0.3 Na-GTP (pH 7.4). Patch pipettes (3~5 MΩ) were pulled on a Narishige electrode puller PC-10 (Narishige, Tokyo, Japan) from Kimax borosilicate glass capillaries (TW150-4, World precision Instruments, Inc., Sarasota, FL, USA) with an inner diameter of 1.12 mm and an outer diameter of 1.5 mm. The analog signal was digitized by an A/D converter (Digidataolecular Devices) and collected with pClamp10.7 software (Molecular Devices). Currents were Bessel-filtered at a cut-off frequency of 5 kHz and digitally sampled at 10 kHz. Including measuring resting membrane potential, all recording was performed in nACSF.
The neurons in mPFC were divided into two types, pyramidal neuron and interneuron. Pyramidal neuron which has a triangular shape is relatively large in size (Cm,120~140 pF), and regulated by interneuron, whereas interneuron which has a circular shape is relatively small in size (Cm, 25~30 pF), and has the ability to control pyramidal neuron [20]. To characterize pyramidal neurons and interneurons, we determined intrinsic properties of neuron, such as input resistance, membrane capacitance, membrane resistance of neuron, and resting membrane potential (RMP) values (Table 1). Data were presented as mean±standard error of the mean and analyzed using Prism 8 software (GraphPad Software Inc, USA). For multiple comparisons, one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc Tukey’s multiple comparison test, and two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used. Asterisks indicate statistically significant differences among the groups.
We compared the differences between the fear-related behaviors of single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats using the passive avoidance test to determine the effect of pair-exposed social buffering condition on electric shock-induced fear response. The schematic timeline of the experimental procedures is presented in Fig. 1A.
In the retention session, the duration of freezing behavior (Fig. 1B; Single, 156.7±33.95,
Studies have suggested that the mPFC is a critical area in the circuitry that controls social behavior [21], and the PL of the mPFC is known as a modulatory area for fear memory conditioning [22]. We assessed the effect of the pair-exposed circumstance on the electrophysiological properties of neurons in the PL of the mPFC to determine whether the pair-exposed social condition modulated mPFC activity.
We used current-clamp patch recording protocol to measure intrinsic electrophysiological properties and observe spikes in pyramidal neurons and fast-spiking (FS) interneurons. There was not significantly different resting membrane potential among three groups in pyramidal neurons and interneurons (Table 1). We estimated I~V curve to compare electric properties and neuron types among assigned groups (Fig. 2A). We measured input resistance (IR), membrane capacitance (Cm) and membrane resistance (Rm) of pyramidal neurons and interneurons in single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats. Cell type had a significant effect (F (1,131)=57.18, p<0.0001) and the effects of IR (F (2,131)=6.24, p<0.01) and cell type×IR interaction (F (2,131)=4.62, p<0.05) also had a significant effect, as determined by the two-way ANOVA. The IR of pyramidal neurons significantly decreased in pair-exposed rats compared to naïve rats, and the IR of interneuron significantly decreased in single-exposed rats compared to pair-exposed and naïve rats (Table 1). Cell type had a significant effect (F (1,113)=90.53, p<0.0001), but the effects of Cm (F (2,113)=0.85, p>0.05) and cell type×Cm interaction (F (2,113)=1.68, p>0.05) were not significant, as determined by the two-way ANOVA. The Cm of interneurons significantly decreased compared to that of the pyramidal neurons, but there was no significant difference among assigned groups (Table 1). The effect of cell type was significant (F (1, 113)=29.88; p<0.0001), but the effects of Rm (F (2, 113)=2.42; p=0.09) and cell type×Rm interaction (F (2, 113)=2.19; p=0.116) were not significant, as determined by the two-way ANOVA. The Rm of interneurons significantly increased compared to that of the pyramidal neurons, and the pyramidal neurons increased significantly in naïve rats compared to single- and pair-exposed rats (Table 1).
The number of action potential (AP) spikes in the pyramidal neurons and interneurons were investigated among single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats. The number of AP spikes in pyramidal neurons revealed significant differences between in pair-exposed rats and naïve rats at 100 and 200 pA current injection (Fig. 2B
To determine the alteration in the oxytocin-induced patterns in the pyramidal neurons, we compared the alteration in the number of AP spikes induced by oxytocin among single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats. Because when oxytocin treated, there showed both patterns of change in the number of spikes, increase and decrease pattern [18, 23, 24], we differentiated and analyzed the pattern of oxytocin-induced spikes and checked the dominant pattern compared between single- and pair-exposed rats. Two patterns of oxytocin-induced changes were observed in pyramidal neurons in assigned rats: increase and decrease (Fig. 3).
In single-exposed rats, 10 of the 25 cells showed an increasing pattern (Control, 20.7±4.7; Oxytocin, 22.3±5.19;
The oxytocin-induced patterns in interneurons revealed patterns of increase and decrease in single- and pair-exposed rats (Fig. 4). In single-exposed rats, 4 of the 16 cells showed an increasing pattern (Control, 113.5±20.89; Oxytocin, 121±22.33;
These results show that the number of AP in the interneurons decreased in single-exposed rats by oxytocin, whereas this pattern by oxytocin was not corresponding in pair-exposed rats, suggesting that oxytocin is regulated in different ways in the interneurons of single- and pair-exposed rats.
The difference between the oxytocin-induced alteration in the number of AP spikes between single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats was compared to that of the controls to statistically clarify the change in AP spikes induced by oxytocin. Pyramidal neurons showed an oxytocin-induced reduction in the number of AP spikes in all assigned rats (Left, Pyramidal neuron: Single, −2.12±0.97,
In summary, pair-exposed rats exhibited a decrease in the number of AP spikes in the pyramidal neurons compared with single-exposed rats, suggesting that decreased neuronal activity elicited by social interaction induced a decreased fear response. Moreover, in interneurons of pair-exposed rats, the change of the number of AP spikes and an increase of input resistance also might possibly contribute to fear reduction. The different oxytocin effect of interneuron in pair-exposed rats compared with single-exposed rats and naïve rats would offer the possibility that oxytocin plays a key role in social buffering reducing the fear response (Fig. 6).
This study verified the positive role of social buffering using the pair-exposed model, and determined its underlying mechanisms using the passive avoidance test and whole-cell patch current-clamp recording. A reduction in freezing behavior and increase in black-room preference was observed in the pair-exposed condition in the retention session of the behavioral test, suggesting that social interaction and social buffering (through pairing) induced a reduction in fear memory formation. The number of AP spikes in the interneurons in mPFC of single- and pair-exposed rats was not significantly different, but a significant decrease was observed in the number of AP spikes in the pyramidal neurons of pair-exposed rats. A decrease in the number of AP spikes in the pyramidal neurons of pair-exposed rats is suggestive of a social interaction effect via pairing, which is consistent with previous research that reported that the excitation/inhibition balance maintained by the FS interneurons in the mPFC is essential for social interaction and sociability [25]. The decrease in the activity of the pyramidal neurons reduced the activity of BLA in the mPFC-BLA circuit, which may have resulted in a decrease in the fear response. Moreover, the oxytocin-induced AP modulation in interneurons of pair-exposed rats was different compared to that of single-exposed rats and naïve rat (Fig. 6). We opine that oxytocin plays a key role in social interaction, and predict the possibility that single-exposed rats would act similar to pair-exposed rats, i.e., exposure to oxytocin in the mPFC would reduce the fear response in single-exposed rats. Therefore, it is necessary to determine the role of oxytocin in single-exposed rats in future studies.
Social animal models can be divided into social buffering with positive effects and social contagion with negative effects [26]. Also, it can be affected their familiar or housing condition. In this study, the familiar pair-exposed condition was less likely to form the fear memory than the single-exposed condition after the application of electric shock. Therefore, the pair-exposed condition in this study provided more of a social buffering effect than a social contagion effect. Previous studies have also shown that the application of social buffering to the fear expression session or learning session reduces the animals’ freezing behavior compared to the isolated condition [27, 28]. Therefore, we think that social buffering not only reduces the formation of fear memory but can also affect the increase in fear extinction.
Oxytocin, which is a social hormone, is one of the social buffering factors that mitigate stress [3]. In human studies, the provision of social support and oxytocin exposure resulted in higher levels of calmness and decreased anxiety [29]. Moreover, it is thought that oxytocin action, via the mPFC, suppresses neuronal activity related to fear memory and activates neuronal activity related to fear extinction [30]. Hence, oxytocin plays an important role in fear memory in the mPFC and is, thus, vital to the study of the mechanism of action of oxytocin in the mPFC.
The E/I balance in the mPFC is thought to be a crucial mechanism for the functioning of the mPFC. E/I imbalance can inhibit the connection between the mediodorsal thalamus and mPFC, thereby inducing abnormalities in sociability [21]. Another study suggested that social behavior deficits improved with an increase in the activity of FS interneurons and a decrease in the activity of pyramidal neurons, which achieved the E/I balance [31]. E/I imbalance can result in diseases such as autism and schizophrenia and is thought to play a role in memory formation or in the persistence of the disturbance mechanism. This study suggests another mechanism that can interfere with memory formation against new negative stimuli.
The mPFC consists of two types of neurons: pyramidal neurons and interneurons. Generally, GABAergic interneurons that regulate pyramidal neurons are classified into regular spike nonpyramidal, FS interneurons, and low-threshold spiking (LTS) interneurons [32, 33]. FS interneurons (former name: PV interneuron) and LTS interneurons (former name: somatostatin interneurons) can be distinguished by the shape of AP spike [34]. FS interneurons are thought to play an important role in social interaction; hence, they are strongly expected to contribute to the effects of social buffering [25]. FS interneurons were mainly targeted in this study; our results suggest that the pattern difference in interneurons represents the difference in the functionality of the mPFC itself, resulting in a change in the fear behavior. However, it is necessary to differentiate between the different types of interneurons and determine their mechanisms in detail in future studies.
Several factors can indicate a change in the number of AP spikes. The first is the change in the resting membrane potential (RMP). A change in the number of AP spikes due to changes in the RMP caused by oxytocin would be expected; however, significant differences were not observed in the RMP of the assigned rats in this study. Other studies reported that the number of AP spikes increased after oxytocin exposure, while no change was observed in the RMP [24]. Second, oxytocin may have changed the threshold of the AP, which may have indicated a change in the number of spikes. Other studies reported that injection of the oxytocin agonist TGOT into the anterior olfactory nucleus neurons resulted in a reduced threshold for the increase in the spike rate [35]. Finally, oxytocin can induce the influx of calcium through various channels, which can result in a change in the AP, if calcium is introduced through channels, such as N-type voltage-gated calcium channels or transient receptor potential vanilloid (TRPV) 2 and TRPV4, among others [36, 37]. Such oxytocin-induced changes in the AP spike have a variety of causes, and future verification of this aspect will determine the mechanism by which oxytocin may have affected the AP spike.
In summary, this study confirmed that the formation of the fear response and fear memory was reduced under the pair-exposed condition, i.e., social buffering, while the number of AP spikes in pair-exposed rats decreased in the PL of the mPFC compared to that in single-exposed rats. Future studies are needed to determine how the coordination between the mPFC-BLA circuit connections affects social buffering in the fear condition, thereby identifying social buffering mechanisms at the neural network level. The synaptic level of E/I balance must be demonstrated to clarify the role of interneurons in social buffering in detail. We also think that the direct association between oxytocin and social buffering presented in this study can be clearly determined if the modification of behavioral patterns by social buffering via treatment with oxytocin agonists and antagonists is identified
The present research was conducted by the research fund of Dankook University in 2018.
Electrophysiological properties of pyramidal and interneurons in single-, pair-exposed rats and naïve rats in mPFC
Pyramidal | Interneuron | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Single | Pair | Naïve | Single | Pair | Naïve | |
Cm (pA) | 58.92±4.17 ( | 67.78±4.73 ( | 56.42±3.73 ( | 24.83±1.74 ( | 27.02±0.9 ( | 30.07±3.49 ( |
Rm (MΩ) | 94.23±8.99** ( | 103.25±16.9* ( | 209.29±24.78 ( | 273.5±56.75 ( | 284.28±35.37 ( | 282.04±41.32 ( |
IR (MΩ) | 140±10 ( | 120±6* ( | 180±8 ( | 190±10* ( | 260±30## ( | 250±10 ( |
RMP (mV) | -74.79±2.7 ( | -70.58±1.54 ( | -73±3.01 ( | -65.63±2.82 ( | -64.12±2.28 ( | -66.43±3.24 ( |
mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; IR, input resistance; Cm, membrane capacitance; Rm, membrane resistance; RMP, resting membrane potential. Two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni post-hoc test: *p<0.05, **p<0.01, compared with Naïve; ## p<0.01, compared with Single.